Why Your Name MattersA) In 194

游客2024-01-20  30

问题                                                 Why Your Name Matters
A)  In 1948, two professors at Harvard University published a study of thirty-three hundred men who had recently graduated, looking at whether their names had any bearing on their academic performance. The men with unusual names, the study found, were more likely to have flunked out (因不及格而退学) or to have exhibited symptoms of psychological neurosis than those with more common names. The Mikes were doing just fine, but the Berriens were having trouble. A rare name, the professors surmised (推测), had a negative psychological effect on its bearer.
B)  Since then, researchers have continued to study the effects of names, and, in the decades after the 1948 study, these findings have been widely reproduced. Some recent research suggests that names can influence choice of profession, where we live, whom we marry, the grades we earn, the stocks we invest in, whether we’re accepted to a school or are hired for a particular job, and the quality of our work in a group setting. Our names can even determine whether we give money to disaster victims: if we share an initial with the name of a hurricane, according to one study, we are far more likely to donate to relief funds after it hits.
C)  Much of the apparent influence of names on behavior has been attributed to what’s known as the implicit-egotism effect; we are generally drawn to the things and people that most resemble us. Because we value and identify with our own names and initials, we prefer things that have something in common with them.
D)  That view, however, may not withstand closer scrutiny. The psychologist Uri Simonsohn has questioned many of the studies that claim to demonstrate the implicit-egotism effect, arguing that the findings are statistical flukes (侥幸) that arise from poor methodology. "It’s like a magician," Simonsohn told me. "He shows you a trick, and you say, ’I know it’s not real, but how did he pull it off?’ It’s all in the methodology." A problem that he cites in some of these studies is an ignorance of base rates—the over-all frequency with which something, like a name, occurs in the population at large. It may be appealing to think that someone named Dan would prefer to be a doctor, but we have to ask whether there are so many doctor Dans simply because Dan is a common name, well-represented in many professions. If that’s the case, the implicit-egotism effect is no longer valid.
E)  There are also researchers who have been more measured in their assessments of the link between name and life outcome. In 1984, the psychologist Debra Crisp and her colleagues found that though more common names were better liked, they had no impact on a person’s educational achievement. In 2012, the psychologists Hui Bai and Kathleen Briggs concluded that "the name initial is at best a very limited unconscious prime, if any." While a person’s name may unconsciously influence his or her thinking, its effects on decision-making are limited. Follow-up studies have also questioned the link between names and longevity, career choice and success, geographic and marriage preferences, and academic achievement.
F)   However, it may not be the case that name effects don’t exist; perhaps they just need to be reinterpreted. In 2004, the economists Marianne Bertrand and Sendhil Mullainathan created five thousand resumes in response to job ads posted in the classifieds in Chicago and Boston newspapers. Using Massachusetts birth certificates from between 1974 and 1979, Bertrand and Mullainathan determined which names appeared at a high frequency in one race but at a low frequency in another, creating groups of what they termed "white-sounding names" (like Emily Walsh and Greg Baker) and " black-sounding names" (like Lakisha Washington and Jamal Jones). They also created two types of candidates; a higher-quality group with more experience and a more complete profile, and a lower-quality group with some obvious gaps in employment or background. They sent two resumes from each qualification group to every employer, one with "black-sounding" name and the other with a "white-sounding" one (a total of four CVs per employer). They found that the "white-sounding" candidates received fifty percent more callbacks, and that the advantage a resume with a "white-sounding" name had over a resume with a "black-sounding" name was roughly equivalent to eight more years of work experience. An average of one of every ten "white" resumes received a callback, versus one of every fifteen "black" resumes. Names, in other words, send signals about who we are and where we come from.
G)  The effects of name-signalling—what names say about ethnicity, religion, social sphere, and socioeconomic background—may begin long before someone enters the workforce. In a study of children in a Florida school district, conducted between 1994 and 2001, the economist David Figlio demonstrated that a child’s name influenced how he or she was treated by the teacher, and that differential treatment, in turn, translated to test scores. Figlio isolated the effects of the students’ names by comparing siblings—same background, different names. Children with names that were linked to low socioeconomic status or being black, as measured by the approach used by Bertrand and Mullainathan, were met with lower teacher expectations. Unsurprisingly, they then performed more poorly than their counterparts with non-black, higher-status names. Conversely, children with Asian-sounding names (also measured by birth-record frequency) were met with higher expectations, and were more frequently placed in gifted programs.
H) The economists Steven Levitt and Roland Fryer looked at trends in names given to black children in the United States from the 1970s to the early 1980s. They discovered that names which sounded more distinctively "black" became, over time, ever more reliable signals of socioeconomic status. That status, in turn, affected a child’s subsequent life outcome, which meant that it was possible to see a correlation between names and outcomes, suggesting a name effect similar to what was observed in the 1948 Harvard study. But when Levitt and Fryer controlled the child’s background, the name effect disappeared, strongly indicating that outcomes weren’t influenced by intrinsic qualities of the name itself. As Simonsohn notes, "Names tell us a lot about who you are. "
I) We see a name, implicitly associate different characteristics with it, and use that association, however unknowingly, to make unrelated judgments about the competence and suitability of its bearer. The relevant question may not be "What’s in a name?" but, rather, "What signals does my name send—and what does it imply?" [br] The implicit-egotism effect may explain why we like things that have something in common with our names or initials.

选项

答案 C

解析 由题干中的implicit—egotism effect和have something in common定位至c)段。细节归纳题。C)段就内隐自我主义效应进行了解释,所谓的内隐自我主义效应就是我们喜欢与我们有相似之处的东西。因为我们注重并认同自己的名字和名字首字母,所以我们喜欢与之有共同之处的东西。由此可归纳出答案是C)。
转载请注明原文地址:https://tihaiku.com/zcyy/3380027.html
最新回复(0)