首页
登录
职称英语
Why Your Name MattersA) In 194
Why Your Name MattersA) In 194
游客
2024-01-20
19
管理
问题
Why Your Name Matters
A) In 1948, two professors at Harvard University published a study of thirty-three hundred men who had recently graduated, looking at whether their names had any bearing on their academic performance. The men with unusual names, the study found, were more likely to have flunked out (因不及格而退学) or to have exhibited symptoms of psychological neurosis than those with more common names. The Mikes were doing just fine, but the Berriens were having trouble. A rare name, the professors surmised (推测), had a negative psychological effect on its bearer.
B) Since then, researchers have continued to study the effects of names, and, in the decades after the 1948 study, these findings have been widely reproduced. Some recent research suggests that names can influence choice of profession, where we live, whom we marry, the grades we earn, the stocks we invest in, whether we’re accepted to a school or are hired for a particular job, and the quality of our work in a group setting. Our names can even determine whether we give money to disaster victims: if we share an initial with the name of a hurricane, according to one study, we are far more likely to donate to relief funds after it hits.
C) Much of the apparent influence of names on behavior has been attributed to what’s known as the implicit-egotism effect; we are generally drawn to the things and people that most resemble us. Because we value and identify with our own names and initials, we prefer things that have something in common with them.
D) That view, however, may not withstand closer scrutiny. The psychologist Uri Simonsohn has questioned many of the studies that claim to demonstrate the implicit-egotism effect, arguing that the findings are statistical flukes (侥幸) that arise from poor methodology. "It’s like a magician," Simonsohn told me. "He shows you a trick, and you say, ’I know it’s not real, but how did he pull it off?’ It’s all in the methodology." A problem that he cites in some of these studies is an ignorance of base rates—the over-all frequency with which something, like a name, occurs in the population at large. It may be appealing to think that someone named Dan would prefer to be a doctor, but we have to ask whether there are so many doctor Dans simply because Dan is a common name, well-represented in many professions. If that’s the case, the implicit-egotism effect is no longer valid.
E) There are also researchers who have been more measured in their assessments of the link between name and life outcome. In 1984, the psychologist Debra Crisp and her colleagues found that though more common names were better liked, they had no impact on a person’s educational achievement. In 2012, the psychologists Hui Bai and Kathleen Briggs concluded that "the name initial is at best a very limited unconscious prime, if any." While a person’s name may unconsciously influence his or her thinking, its effects on decision-making are limited. Follow-up studies have also questioned the link between names and longevity, career choice and success, geographic and marriage preferences, and academic achievement.
F) However, it may not be the case that name effects don’t exist; perhaps they just need to be reinterpreted. In 2004, the economists Marianne Bertrand and Sendhil Mullainathan created five thousand resumes in response to job ads posted in the classifieds in Chicago and Boston newspapers. Using Massachusetts birth certificates from between 1974 and 1979, Bertrand and Mullainathan determined which names appeared at a high frequency in one race but at a low frequency in another, creating groups of what they termed "white-sounding names" (like Emily Walsh and Greg Baker) and " black-sounding names" (like Lakisha Washington and Jamal Jones). They also created two types of candidates; a higher-quality group with more experience and a more complete profile, and a lower-quality group with some obvious gaps in employment or background. They sent two resumes from each qualification group to every employer, one with "black-sounding" name and the other with a "white-sounding" one (a total of four CVs per employer). They found that the "white-sounding" candidates received fifty percent more callbacks, and that the advantage a resume with a "white-sounding" name had over a resume with a "black-sounding" name was roughly equivalent to eight more years of work experience. An average of one of every ten "white" resumes received a callback, versus one of every fifteen "black" resumes. Names, in other words, send signals about who we are and where we come from.
G) The effects of name-signalling—what names say about ethnicity, religion, social sphere, and socioeconomic background—may begin long before someone enters the workforce. In a study of children in a Florida school district, conducted between 1994 and 2001, the economist David Figlio demonstrated that a child’s name influenced how he or she was treated by the teacher, and that differential treatment, in turn, translated to test scores. Figlio isolated the effects of the students’ names by comparing siblings—same background, different names. Children with names that were linked to low socioeconomic status or being black, as measured by the approach used by Bertrand and Mullainathan, were met with lower teacher expectations. Unsurprisingly, they then performed more poorly than their counterparts with non-black, higher-status names. Conversely, children with Asian-sounding names (also measured by birth-record frequency) were met with higher expectations, and were more frequently placed in gifted programs.
H) The economists Steven Levitt and Roland Fryer looked at trends in names given to black children in the United States from the 1970s to the early 1980s. They discovered that names which sounded more distinctively "black" became, over time, ever more reliable signals of socioeconomic status. That status, in turn, affected a child’s subsequent life outcome, which meant that it was possible to see a correlation between names and outcomes, suggesting a name effect similar to what was observed in the 1948 Harvard study. But when Levitt and Fryer controlled the child’s background, the name effect disappeared, strongly indicating that outcomes weren’t influenced by intrinsic qualities of the name itself. As Simonsohn notes, "Names tell us a lot about who you are. "
I) We see a name, implicitly associate different characteristics with it, and use that association, however unknowingly, to make unrelated judgments about the competence and suitability of its bearer. The relevant question may not be "What’s in a name?" but, rather, "What signals does my name send—and what does it imply?" [br] Professors at Harvard University found that men with peculiar names did not perform academically as well as those with ordinary names.
选项
答案
A
解析
由题干中的Professors at Harvard University和men with peculiar names定位至A)段第一、二句。细节推断题。A)段前两句提到。哈佛大学的两名教授发现名字独特的人比名字普通的人更容易因考试不及格而被迫退学,由此可推断,名字独特的人在学业成绩方面不如名字普通的人好,因此答案是A)。
转载请注明原文地址:https://tihaiku.com/zcyy/3380022.html
相关试题推荐
WhyYourNameMattersA)In194
WhyYourNameMattersA)In194
WhyYourNameMattersA)In194
WhyYourNameMattersA)In194
WhyYourNameMattersA)In194
WhyYourNameMattersA)In194
WhyYourNameMattersA)In194
WhyYourNameMattersA)In194
WhyYourNameMattersA)In194
WhyYourNameMattersA)In194
随机试题
某公路项目,建设单位通过公开招标确定了施工单位和监理单位并分别签订了合同。施工合
从所给的四个选项中,选择最合适的一个填入问号处。使之呈现一定的规律性: A.如
患儿,女,8岁。诊断为急性白血病入院。体检:体温40.1℃,咽痛。血红蛋白75g
因基金管理人的过失行为给基金资产或基金份额持有人造成损害的,基金托管人应该()
控制高血压A.碳酸氢钠 B.呋塞米 C.ACEI类 D.重组人促红素 E
下列关于证券交收违约处理的描述中不正确的是()。A:对于证券交收违约,中国结算公
药品说明书和标签,是药品外在质量的主要体现,是传递药品信息,指导医师用药和消费者
下列选项中,不属于劳动争议处理的一般程序的是()。A.协商 B.搁置 C.
下列各项费用可以计入材料单价的有( )。A.材料的供应价格 B.材料的检验试验
香豆素类药物的抗凝作用机制是A.激活血浆中的ATⅢ B.抑制凝血酶原转变成凝血
最新回复
(
0
)