首页
登录
职称英语
By now, it should come as no surprise when scientists discover yet another c
By now, it should come as no surprise when scientists discover yet another c
游客
2024-12-25
6
管理
问题
By now, it should come as no surprise when scientists discover yet another case of experience changing the brain. From the sensory information we absorb to the movements we make, our lives leave footprints on the bumps and fissures of our cortex, so much so that experiences can alter "hard-wired" brain structures. Through rehab, stroke patients can coax a region of the motor cortex on the opposite side of the damaged region to pinch-hit, restoring lost mobility; volunteers who are blindfolded for just five days can reprogram their visual cortex to process sound and touch.
Still, scientists have been surprised at how deeply culture—the language we speak, the values we absorb—shapes the brain, and are rethinking findings derived from studies of Westerners. To take one recent example, a region behind the forehead called the medial prefrontal cortex supposedly represents the self: it is active when we ("we" being the Americans in the study) think of our own identity and traits. But with Chinese volunteers, the results were strikingly different. The "me" circuit hummed not only when they thought whether a particular adjective described themselves, but also when they considered whether it described their mother. The Westerners showed no such overlap between self and mom. Depending whether one lives in a culture that views the self as autonomous and unique or as connected to and part of a larger whole, this neural circuit takes on quite different functions.
"Cultural neuroscience," as this new field is called, is about discovering such differences. Some of the findings, as with the "me/mom" circuit, buttress longstanding notions of cultural differences. For instance, it is a cultural cliche that Westerners focus on individual objects while East Asians pay attention to context and background (another manifestation of the individualism-collectivism split). Sure enough, when shown complex, busy scenes, Asian-Americans and non-Asian—Americans recruited different brain regions. The Asians showed more activity in areas that process figure-ground relations—holistic context—while the Americans showed more activity in regions that recognize objects.
Psychologist Nalini Ambady of Tufts found something similar when she and colleagues showed drawings of people in a submissive pose (head down, shoulders hunched) or a dominant one (arms crossed, face forward) to Japanese and Americans. The brain’s dopamine-fueled reward circuit became most active at the sight of the stance—dominant for Americans, submissive for Japanese—that each volunteer’s culture most values, they reported in 2009. This raises an obvious chicken-and-egg question.
Cultural neuroscience wouldn’t be making waves if it found neurobiological bases only for well-known cultural differences. It is also uncovering the unexpected. For instance, a 2006 study found that native Chinese speakers use a different region of the brain to do simple arithmetic (3 + 4) or decide which number is larger than native English speakers do, even though both use Arabic numerals. The Chinese use the circuits that process visual and spatial information and plan movements (the latter may be related to the use of the abacus). But English speakers use language circuits. It is as if the West conceives numbers as just words, but the East imbues them with symbolic, spatial freight. "One would think that neural processes involving basic mathematical computations are universal," says Ambady, but they "seem to be culture-specific. "
Not to be the skunk at this party, but I think it’s important to ask whether neuroscience reveals anything more than we already know from, say, anthropology. For instance, it’s well known that East Asian cultures prize the collective over the individual, and that Americans do the opposite.
Ambady thinks cultural neuroscience does advance understanding. Take the me/mom finding, which, she argues, "attests to the strength of the overlap between self and people close to you in collectivistic cultures and the separation in individualistic cultures. It is important to push the analysis to the level of the brain. " Especially when it shows how fundamental cultural differences are—so fundamental, perhaps, that "universal" notions such as human rights, democracy, and the like may be no such thing. [br] We can know from the first paragraph that______.
选项
A、the assumption that human experience can change human brain structure has already been widely proved
B、human experience can change brain structure
C、stroke patients can restore mobility by themselves
D、people blindfolded for several days can still have visual ability
答案
B
解析
根据原文第一段中第一句,我们不能由此推断出“人类经历改变人类大脑结构”这一概念得到了广泛的论证;根据原文第一段中第二句,我们得知人类经历可以改变人类大脑结构;根据原文第一段中第三句,我们不能由此推断出中风病人可以自行恢复活动能力;根据原文第一段中最后一句,我们得知被遮蔽眼睛的人可以在几天之后调节视觉皮层使其适应新的状况。
转载请注明原文地址:https://tihaiku.com/zcyy/3883560.html
相关试题推荐
Anothermethodtoanalyzeasentencefromthefunctionalperspectiveis______,
Bynow,itshouldcomeasnosurprisewhenscientistsdiscoveryetanotherc
Bynow,itshouldcomeasnosurprisewhenscientistsdiscoveryetanotherc
Bynow,itshouldcomeasnosurprisewhenscientistsdiscoveryetanotherc
Bynow,itshouldcomeasnosurprisewhenscientistsdiscoveryetanotherc
Virtuallyeverydayoftheyearseesanotherartbiennialopeningsomewhere
Virtuallyeverydayoftheyearseesanotherartbiennialopeningsomewhere
Whichofthefollowingreasonsdoscientistssupportthatcanexplaintheextinc
WhatarethescientistsattheUniversityofCaliforniadoingtohelppatientsb
Sep.11deliveredbothashockandasurprise—theattack,andourresponset
随机试题
A.各实验组与对照组在非实验因素方面尽可能一致 B.每一实验单位具有同等机会被
水泥中不同矿物水化速率有较大差别,因此可以通过其在水泥中的相对含量来满足不同工程
林某,男,65岁,近1周来出现入睡困难失眠。经询问,张某平时没有午休习惯,晚餐后
患者男,70岁,帕金森病史8年,间断服用苯海索治疗。近1个月病情加重,吞咽困难,
可燃气体探测报警系统 某在建天然气厂设计了一套天然气探测报警系统
下列选项中属于薪酬制度中常见问题的是( )。A.岗位价值没有量化,薪酬的内部公
房地产开发项目的资本金占项目总投资的比例不得低于()。A:10% B:20%
根据我国《公司法》,在有限责任公司和股份有限公司监事会组成中,职工代表的比例不得
阿司匹林长期或大剂量使用可引起A、凝血障碍 B、水钠潴留,可致水肿 C、胃癌
线性组织结构模式的特点之一是()。A.组织内每个工作部门可能有多个矛盾的指令源
最新回复
(
0
)