首页
登录
职称英语
By now, it should come as no surprise when scientists discover yet another c
By now, it should come as no surprise when scientists discover yet another c
游客
2023-12-14
31
管理
问题
By now, it should come as no surprise when scientists discover yet another case of experience changing the brain. From the sensory information we absorb to the movements we make, our lives leave footprints on the bumps and fissures of our cortex, so much so that experiences can alter "hard-wired" brain structures. Through rehab, stroke patients can coax a region of the motor cortex on the opposite side of the damaged region to pinch-hit, restoring lost mobility; volunteers who are blindfolded for just five days can reprogram their visual cortex to process sound and touch.
Still, scientists have been surprised at how deeply culture—the language we speak, the values we absorb—shapes the brain, and are rethinking findings derived from studies of Westerners. To take one recent example, a region behind the forehead called the medial prefrontal cortex supposedly represents the self: it is active when we ("we" being the Americans in the study) think of our own identity and traits. But with Chinese volunteers, the results were strikingly different. The "me" circuit hummed not only when they thought whether a particular adjective described themselves, but also when they considered whether it described their mother. The Westerners showed no such overlap between self and mom. Depending whether one lives in a culture that views the self as autonomous and unique or as connected to and part of a larger whole, this neural circuit takes on quite different functions.
"Cultural neuroscience," as this new field is called, is about discovering such differences. Some of the findings, as with the "me/mom" circuit, buttress longstanding notions of cultural differences. For instance, it is a cultural cliché that Westerners focus on individual objects while East Asians pay attention to context and background (another manifestation of the individualism-collectivism split). Sure enough, when shown complex, busy scenes, Asian-Americans and non-Asian—Americans recruited different brain regions. The Asians showed more activity in areas that process figure-ground relations—holistic context—while the Americans showed more activity in regions that recognize objects.
Psychologist Nalini Ambady of Tufts found something similar when she and colleagues showed drawings of people in a submissive pose (head down, shoulders hunched) or a dominant one (arms crossed, face forward) to Japanese and Americans. The brain’s dopamine-fueled reward circuit became most active at the sight of the stance—dominant for Americans, submissive for Japanese—that each volunteer’s culture most values, they reported in 2009. This raises an obvious chicken-and-egg question.
Cultural neuroscience wouldn’t be making waves if it found neurobiological bases only for well-known cultural differences. It is also uncovering the unexpected. For instance, a 2006 study found that native Chinese speakers use a different region of the brain to do simple arithmetic (3+ 4) or decide which number is larger than native English speakers do, even though both use Arabic numerals. The Chinese use the circuits that process visual and spatial information and plan movements (the latter may be related to the use of the abacus). But English speakers use language circuits. It is as if the West Conceives numbers as just words, but the East imbues them with symbolic, spatial freight. "One would think that neural processes involving basic mathematical computations are universal," says Ambady, but they "seem to be culture-specific. "
Not to be the skunk at this party, but I think it’s important to ask whether neuroscience reveals anything more than we already know from, say, anthropology. For instance, it’s well known that East Asian cultures prize the collective over the individual, and that Americans do the opposite.
Ambady thinks cultural neuroscience does advance understanding. Take the me/mom finding, which, she argues, "attests to the strength of the overlap between self and people close to you in collectivistic cultures and the separation in individualistic cultures. It is important to push the analysis to the level of the brain. " Especially when it shows how fundamental cultural differences are—so fundamental, perhaps, that "universal" notions such as human rights, democracy, and the like may be no such thing. [br] A 2006 study found that______.
选项
A、both the Chinese and Westerners use the same part of the brain to do arithmetic
B、the Chinese use visual and spatial circuits while Westerners use language circuits
C、the Chinese conceive numbers as words while Westerners conceive numbers as symbols
D、neural processes involving basic mathematical computations are universal.
答案
B
解析
根据原文第五段,我们得知东西方人使用不同的大脑区域来进行算术;东方人倾向于视觉加工,而西方人倾向于语言加工;西方人把数字当作语言来加工,而东方人把数字当作视觉标志来进行加工;根据该段最后一句得知,大脑神经运作过程是因文化不同而不同的。
转载请注明原文地址:https://tihaiku.com/zcyy/3272618.html
相关试题推荐
JonasFrisenhadhiseurekamomentin1997.Backthen,scientistssuspectedt
JonasFrisenhadhiseurekamomentin1997.Backthen,scientistssuspectedt
Shoulddoctorseverlietobenefittheirpatients—tospeedrecoveryortocon
Shoulddoctorseverlietobenefittheirpatients—tospeedrecoveryortocon
Shoulddoctorseverlietobenefittheirpatients—tospeedrecoveryortocon
IftheFederationofAmericanScientistsmadealistofeducationalvideogam
IftheFederationofAmericanScientistsmadealistofeducationalvideogam
IftheFederationofAmericanScientistsmadealistofeducationalvideogam
Itisthenewsthatallslothshavebeenwaitingfor.ScientistsinGermanyh
Itisthenewsthatallslothshavebeenwaitingfor.ScientistsinGermanyh
随机试题
Firstofcourse,itisplainthatintheyear2010everyonewillhaveathis
Activitiessuchaseatingandshoppingaregenerallyincludedin"play".[br]Th
WhenDidAidsBegin?Theyea
有防水要求的厕浴间进行防水施工,饰面层完工后应进行第二次蓄水试验,第二次蓄水试验
阅读以下说明和C函数,填补代码中的空缺(1)~(5),将解答填入答题纸的对应栏内
腺泡状软组织肉瘤的特征有:()A.青少年多见 B.多发生于四肢 C.腺泡
某高校测得1100名20岁健康男大学生的身高,经检验资料服从正态分布,其均值为1
各种运输方式内外部的各个方面的构成和联系,就是( )。 A.运输系统
期货公司应当制定防范期货投资咨询业务与其他期货业务之间利益冲突的管理制度,建立健
第二、三类防雷建筑物防直击雷接地装置的要求,下列正确的是()。
最新回复
(
0
)