首页
登录
职称英语
Where does morality come from? Throughout the history of Western civilizatio
Where does morality come from? Throughout the history of Western civilizatio
游客
2023-12-04
23
管理
问题
Where does morality come from? Throughout the history of Western civilization thinkers have usually answered either that it comes from God, or else through die application of reason.
But in The Bonobo and the Atheist, primatologist Frans de Waal argues that there’s another answer that fits the data better: morality comes from our evolutionary past as a social primate(灵长目动物). Like our closest relatives, the apes, humans evolved in small, tightly knit, cooperative groups. As a result, again like the apes, we are exquisitely sensitive to one another’s moods, needs and intentions.
This well-developed empathy provided the trellis(框架)on which morality later flowered. De Waal, who is based at Emory University in Atlanta, Georgia, has been making this case eloquently for many years and over several books, notably in Good Natured back in 1997, and in Primates and Philosophers, 12 years later.
In his new work, he bolsters(支持)the argument by drawing on a lot of new research, carefully footnoted for those who want to dig deeper. De Waal distinguishes two degrees of morality. The first he calls "one-on-one morality", which governs how an individual can expect to be treated, and the second "community concern" , a larger, more abstract concept that extends to the harmony of the group as a whole.
Chimps and bonobos certainly have the former—they respect ownership, for example, and expect to be treated according to their place in the hierarchy. But de Waal presents several examples—such as a chimp stepping in to stop a fight between two others—that suggest that they also have a rudimentary(初步的)form of the latter.
The book’s title, incidentally, draws on bonobos because they are more likely than chimps to behave morally, to have concern for each other, to value harmony and so on. This, imagines, de Waal, is something morally inclined atheists would want to emulate. If humans inherited morality from our ancestors, though, what are we to make of religion? Here de Waal moves into the territory he has not explored before. Clearly, religion must do something important, since every human culture has it. But instead of religion giving us morality, de Waal turns the tables. Morality, he argues, probably gave us religion as a way of reinforcing the pre-existing community concern.
If he’s right, then there may be no absolute code of right and wrong out there to be discovered. Instead, each individual’s evolved sense of empathy and concern for the group may help shape the group’s consensus on what kind of behaviour is appropriate. In short, says de Waal, morality may be something we all have to work out together. It’s a persuasive argument, and de Waal’s cautious and evidence-based approach is one that many New Scientist readers are sure to find congenial.
That careful approach is less evident in another book covering some of the same ground. In How Animals Grieve, anthropologist Barbara King sets out to explore the question of whether non-human animals grieve for their dead. It’s an intriguing question, but unfortunately King’s book is largely a succession of anecdotes: the cat who roams the house, crying, in search of its dead litter mate: the dog who waits daily at the train station for its dead master: a dolphin trying to keep her dead calf afloat for days.
Some of these stories make a persuasive case for some animals—especially apes, elephants and cetaceans— sometimes grieving. No surprises there: I suspect most readers would have conceded that ground right from the start.
But King makes little effort to dig any deeper by exploring, for example, the neural machinery and cognitive skills an animal needs in order to be capable of grief. After all, solitary species such as cats have less need for empathy—and its corollary, grief—than social animals, and small-brained creatures such as turtles may simply lack the brainpower or not form lasting pair bonds.
To his credit, de Waal takes full note of such distinctions: King, not so much. [br] Which statement would Frans de Waal most probably agree to?
选项
A、Bonobos are not as concerned about each other as chimps.
B、Atheists would like to emulate the harmony among Bonobos.
C、Bonobos are a good example to show where morality comes from.
D、Atheists are a group of people who are lack of moral standards.
答案
C
解析
细节题。作者在第六段第一句提到Frans de Waal的书时指出,书名用倭黑猩猩(bonobos)是因为它们比黑猩猩(chimps)在行为上表现得更道德,可见Frans de Waal认为,倭黑猩猩是说明道德来源的好例子,故[C]为答案。
转载请注明原文地址:https://tihaiku.com/zcyy/3242610.html
相关试题推荐
Themonarchof______ruledBritainthelongesttimeinthehistory.A、QueenElizab
Wheredoesmoralitycomefrom?ThroughoutthehistoryofWesterncivilizatio
Wheredoesmoralitycomefrom?ThroughoutthehistoryofWesterncivilizatio
Wheredoesmoralitycomefrom?ThroughoutthehistoryofWesterncivilizatio
Wheredoesmoralitycomefrom?ThroughoutthehistoryofWesterncivilizatio
Thestudyofalanguagethroughthecourseofitshistoryiscalled______A、Pers
______isNOTwrittenbyWashingtonIrving.A、AHistoryofNewYorkB、TheLegendo
Throughoutthe1600s,______foundedpermanentsettlementsinCanada.A、Englandan
InthehistoryofGreatBritain,mostpeoplehavelivedinthe______withamilde
ThefirstimmigrantsinAmericanhistorycamefrom______.A、EnglandB、theNetherl
随机试题
关于建设项目前期咨询评估的说法,正确的是( )。(2016年真题)A:项目建议书
属于肾蒂的结构是()A.肾动脉 B.肾小盏 C.肾大盏 D.输尿管 E
教育规划的制定方法()。 Ⅰ.确定教育目标 Ⅱ.计算教育资金需求 Ⅲ.
A.抗血细胞抗体 B.循环免疫复合物 C.皮肤试验 D.过敏原和IgE
停用防误闭锁装置应经地市公司(省检修公司)、县公司分管生产的行政副职或总工程师批
上百种货物整齐有序、错落有致地摆放在货架上,有酒类、玉石类、家居类、鞋帽类等等,
一种具有法律约束力的中期周转性票据发行融资的承诺是( )。A.项目贷款承诺
(2018年真题)下表为2016年10月某交易日,铜期货合约的相关价格信息及标准
(2018年真题)企业在资产减值测试时,下列关于预计资产未来现金流量的表述中,正
根据《招标投标法实施条例》,下列属于投标人相互串通投标情形的是()。A.不
最新回复
(
0
)