首页
登录
职称英语
Do music lessons really make chi
Do music lessons really make chi
游客
2023-08-05
29
管理
问题
Do music lessons really make children smarter?
A) A recent analysis found that most research mischaracterizes the relationship between music and skills enhancement.
B) In 2004, a paper appeared in the journal Psychological Science, titled "Music Lessons Enhance IQ." The author, composer and psychologist Glenn Schellenberg, had conducted an experiment with 144 children randomly assigned to four groups: one learned the keyboard for a year, one took singing lessons, one joined an acting class, and a control group had no extracurricular training. The IQ of the children in the two musical groups rose by an average of seven points in the course of a year; those in the other two groups gained an average of 4.3 points.
C) Schellenberg had long been skeptical of the science supporting claims that music education enhances children’s abstract reasoning, math, or language skills. If children who play the piano are smarter, he says, it doesn’t necessarily mean they are smarter because they play the piano. It could be that the youngsters who play the piano also happen to be more ambitious or better at focusing on a task. Correlation, after all, does not prove causation.
D) The 2004 paper was specifically designed to address those concerns. And as a passionate musician, Schellenberg was delighted when he turned up credible evidence that music has transfer effects on general intelligence. But nearly a decade later, in 2013, the Education Endowment Foundation funded a bigger study with more than 900 students. That study failed to confirm Schellenberg’s findings, producing no evidence that music lessons improved math and literacy skills.
E) Schellenberg took that news in stride while continuing to cast a skeptical eye on the research in his field. Recently, he decided to formally investigate just how often his fellow researchers in psychology and neuroscience make what he believes are erroneous—or at least premature—causal connections between music and intelligence. His results, published in May, suggest that many of his peers do just that.
F) For his recent study, Schellenberg asked two research assistants to look for correlational studies on the effects of music education. They found a total of 114 papers published since 2000. To assess whether the authors claimed any causation, researchers then looked for telltale verbs in each paper’s title and abstract, verbs like "enhance," "promote," "facilitate," and "strengthen." The papers were categorized as neuroscience if the study employed a brain imaging method like magnetic resonance, or if the study appeared in a journal that had "brain," "neuroscience," or a related term in its title. Otherwise the papers were categorized as psychology. Schellenberg didn’t tell his assistants what exactly he was trying to prove.
G) After computing their assessments, Schellenberg concluded that the majority of the articles erroneously claimed that music training had a causal effect. The overselling, he also found, was more prevalent among neuroscience studies, three quarters of which mischaracterized a mere association between music training and skills enhancement as a cause-and-effect relationship. This may come as a surprise to some. Psychologists have been battling charges that they don’t do "real" science for some time—in large part because many findings from classic experiments have proved unreproducible. Neuroscientists, on the other hand, armed with brain scans and EEGs (脑电图), have not been subject to the same degree of critique.
H) To argue for a cause-and-effect relationship, scientists must attempt to explain why and how a connection could occur. When it comes to transfer effects of music, scientists frequently point to brain plasticity—the fact that the brain changes according to how we use it. When a child learns to play the violin, for example, several studies have shown that the brain region responsible for the fine motor skills of the left hand’s fingers is likely to grow. And many experiments have shown that musical training improves certain hearing capabilities, like filtering voices from background noise or distinguishing the difference between the consonants (辅音) ’b’ and ’g’.
I) But Schellenberg remains highly critical of how the concept of plasticity has been applied in his field. "Plasticity has become an industry of its own," he wrote in his May paper. Practice does change the brain, he allows, but what is questionable is the assertion that these changes affect other brain regions, such as those responsible for spatial reasoning or math problems.
J) Neuropsychologist Lutz Jancke agrees. "Most of these studies don’t allow for causal inferences," he said. For over two decades, Jancke has researched the effects of music lessons, and like Schellenberg, he believes that the only way to truly understand their effects is to run longitudinal studies. In such studies, researchers would need to follow groups of children with and without music lessons over a long period of time—even if the assignments are not completely random. Then they could compare outcomes for each group.
K) Some researchers are starting to do just that. The neuroscientist Peter Schneider from Heidelberg University in Germany, for example, has been following a group of children for ten years now. Some of them were handed musical instruments and given lessons through a school-based program in the Ruhr region of Germany called Jedem Kind ein Instrument, or "an instrument for every child," which was carried out with government funding. Among these children, Schneider has found that those who were enthusiastic about music and who practiced voluntarily showed improvements in hearing ability, as well as in more general competencies, such as the ability to concentrate.
L) To establish whether effects such as improved concentration are caused by music participation itself, and not by investing time in an extracurricular activity of any kind, Assal Habibi, a psychology professor at the University of Southern California, is conducting a five-year longitudinal study with children from low-income communities in Los Angeles. The youngsters fall into three groups: those who take after-school music, those who do after-school sports, and those with no structured after-school program at all. After two years, Habibi and her colleagues reported seeing structural changes in the brains of the musically trained children, both locally and in the pathways connecting different parts of the brain.
M)That may seem compelling, but Habibi’s children were not selected randomly. Did the children who were drawn to music perhaps have something in them from the start that made them different but eluded the brain scanners? "As somebody who started taking piano lessons at the age of five and got up every morning at seven to practice, that experience changed me and made me part of who I am today," Schellenberg said. "The question is whether those kinds of experiences do so systematically across individuals and create exactly the same changes. And I think that is that huge leap of faith."
N) Did he have a hidden talent that others didn’t have? Or more endurance than his peers? Music researchers tend, like Schellenberg, to be musicians themselves, and as he noted in his recent paper, "the idea of positive cognitive and neural side effects from music training (and other pleasurable activities) is inherently appealing." He also admits that if he had children of his own, he would encourage them to take music lessons and go to university. "I would think that it makes them better people, more critical, just wiser in general," he said.
O) But those convictions should be checked at the entrance to the lab, he added. Otherwise, the work becomes religion or faith. "You have to let go of your faith if you want to be a scientist." [br] Glenn Schellenberg was doubtful about the claim that music education helps enhance children’s intelligence.
选项
答案
C
解析
注意抓住题干中的关键信息Glenn Schellenberg was doubtful about the claim。原文中有两处提到格伦.舍伦贝格对某事持怀疑态度,分别是在C段开头和E段开头。C段开头说,舍伦贝格长期以来一直对支持音乐教育能提高儿童抽象推理、数学或语言技能等主张的科学持怀疑态度(Skeptical of the science supporting claims…)。意思是说舍伦贝格对音乐教育有助于提高孩子智力这一观点持怀疑态度,与题干的意思一致,故答案为C。题干中的Glenn Schellenberg was doubtful about对应原文中Schellenberg had long been skeptical of;题干中的children’s intelligence是对原文中的children’s abstract reasoning,math,or language skills的概括。E段前句中提到continuing to cast a skeptical eye on the research in his field,说的是舍伦贝格继续对自己领域的研究持怀疑态度,没有明确说是对音乐教育有助于提高孩子智力这一观点的态度,故不选。
转载请注明原文地址:https://tihaiku.com/zcyy/2898429.html
相关试题推荐
[originaltext]Canyoureallymakenewfriendsasanadult?Imean,there’sp
[originaltext]Canyoureallymakenewfriendsasanadult?Imean,there’sp
[originaltext]Canyoureallymakenewfriendsasanadult?Imean,there’sp
[originaltext]Canyoureallymakenewfriendsasanadult?Imean,there’splen
[originaltext]Canyoureallymakenewfriendsasanadult?Imean,there’splen
[originaltext]Canyoureallymakenewfriendsasanadult?Imean,there’s
[originaltext]Canyoureallymakenewfriendsasanadult?Imean,there’s
[originaltext]W:Jason,actuallyI’mgoingtoEnglandthissummer.M:Really?T
[originaltext]M:Hey,I’malittlelowoncashthesedays.W:Really?Whatabou
[originaltext]M:Hey,I’malittlelowoncashthesedays.W:Really?Whatabou
随机试题
Thereaxecockroaches(蟑螂)everywhereonEarthexcepttheplacesthataxecove
Schoolslookingtobancellphonesmayhaveanewexcuse:agrowingnumbero
收费站收费系统的主要构成,按其基本功能可分为()。A.计算机系统 B.收
女,10岁。混合牙列。磨牙中性关系,上颌中度拥挤,下颌轻度拥挤,上牙弓狭窄,双侧
世界上天主教徒最多的国家是()。A.美国 B.英国 C.巴西 D.法国
患者,女,27岁,西医诊断为急性乳腺炎。症见乳房生疮,疮口溃后疼痛逐渐减轻,但脓
SaaS云服务在物业经营管理中的应用包括()。A:SaaS云服务让无形的物业经营
道氏理论认为,价格的波动尽管表现形式不同,但最终可以将它们分为三种趋势()。A:
经济全球化是指商品、服务、生产要素与信息跨国界流动的规模与形式不断增加,通过国际
A.虫蛀 B.霉变 C.酸败 D.沉淀 E.挥发糖浆剂在贮藏中易发生
最新回复
(
0
)