首页
登录
职称英语
On May 24, 1976, the British wine merchant Steven Spurrier organized a blind
On May 24, 1976, the British wine merchant Steven Spurrier organized a blind
游客
2024-12-28
0
管理
问题
On May 24, 1976, the British wine merchant Steven Spurrier organized a blind tasting of French and Californian wines. Spurrier was a Francophile and, like most wine experts, didn’t expect the New World upstarts to compete with the premiers crus from Bordeaux. He assembled a panel of eleven wine experts and had them taste a variety of Cabernets blind, rating each bottle on a twenty-point scale.The results shocked the wine world. According to the judges, the best Cabernet at the tasting was a 1973 bottle from Stag’s Leap Wine Cellars in Napa Valley. When the tasting was repeated a few years later—some judges insisted that the French wines had been drunk too young—Stag’s Leap was once again declared the winner, followed by three other California Cabernets. These blind tastings helped to legitimate Napa vineyards.
But now, in an even more surprising turn of events, another American wine region has performed far better than expected in a blind tasting against the finest French chateaus. Ready for the punch line? The wines were from New Jersey.
The tasting was closely modelled on the 1976 event, featuring the same fancy Bordeaux vineyards, such as Chateau Mouton Rothschild and Chateau Haut-Brion. The Jersey entries included bottles from the Heritage Vineyards in Mullica Hill and Unionville Vineyards in Ringoes. The nine judges were French and American wine experts.
The Judgment of Princeton didn’t quite end with a Jersey victory—a French wine was on top in both the red and white categories—but, in terms of the reassurance for those with valuable wine collections, it might as well have. Clos des Mouches only narrowly beat out Unionville Single Vineyard and two other Jersey whites, while Chateau Mouton Rothschild and Haut-Brion topped Heritage’s BDX. The wines from New Jersey cost, on average, about five percent as much as their French counterparts. And then there’s the inconsistency of the judges: the scores for that Mouton Rothschild, for instance, ranged from 11 to 19.5. On the excellent blog Marginal Revolution, the economist Tyler Cowen highlights the analysis of the Princeton professor Richard Quand, who found that almost of all the wines were "statistically undistinguishable" from each other. This suggests that, if the blind tasting were held again, a Jersey wine might very well win.
What can we learn from these tests? First, that tasting wine is really hard, even for experts. Because the sensory differences between different bottles of rotten grape juice are so slight—and the differences get even more muddled after a few sips—there is often wide disagreement about which wines are best. For instance, both the winning red and white wines in the Princeton tasting were ranked by at least one of the judges as the worst.
The perceptual ambiguity of wine helps explain why contextual influences—say, the look of a label, or the price tag on the bottle—can profoundly influence expert judgment. This was nicely demonstrated in a mischievous 2001 experiment led by Frederic Brochet at the University of Bordeaux. In the first test, Brochet invited fifty-seven wine experts and asked them to give their impressions of what looked like two glasses of red and white wine. The wines were actually the same white wine, one of which had been tinted red with food coloring. But that didn’t stop the experts from describing the "red" wine in language typically used to describe red wines. One expert praised its "jamminess," while another enjoyed its "crushed red fruit."
The second test Brochet conducted was even more damning. He took a middling Bordeaux and served it in two different bottles. One bottle bore the label of a fancy grand cru, the other of an ordinary vin de table. Although they were being served the exact same wine, the experts gave the bottles nearly opposite descriptions. The grand cru was summarized as being "agreeable," "woody," "complex," "balanced," and "rounded," while the most popular adjectives for the vin de table included "weak," "short," "light," " flat," and "faulty."
The results are even more distressing for non-experts. In recent decades, the wine world has become an increasingly quantitative place, as dependent on scores and statistics as Billy Beane. But these ratings suggest a false sense of precision, as if it were possible to reliably identify the difference between an eighty-nine-point Merlot from Jersey and a ninety-one-point blend from Bordeaux—or even a greater spread. And so we linger amid the wine racks, paralyzed by the alcoholic arithmetic. How much are we willing to pay for a few extra points?
These calculations are almost certainly a waste of time. Last year, the psychologist Richard Wiseman bought a wide variety of bottles at the local supermarket, from a five-dollar Bordeaux to a fifty-dollar champagne, and asked people to say which wine was more expensive. (All of the taste tests were conducted double-blind, with neither the experimenter nor subject aware of the actual price.) According to Wiseman’s data, the five hundred and seventy-eight participants could only pick the more expensive wine fifty-three percent of the time, which is basically random chance. They actually performed below chance when it came to picking red wines. Bordeaux fared the worst, with a significant majority—sixty-one percent—picking the cheap plonk as the more expensive selection.
From The New Yorker, June 13, 2012 [br] Which one of the following best explains the intention of the author?
选项
A、It’s impossible for the non-expert to perform well in wine tasting.
B、Wine tasting is a very complicated and meticulous job.
C、There’s a lot of uncertainties in blind tasting.
D、Because of the hardness of wine tasting, labels and price tags turn out to be trustworthy.
答案
B
解析
本题为主旨题。虽然在文章倒数第二段有The results are even more distressing for non—expels.证明选项A是有提到的。但是通篇也讲到了专家们品酒也一样地难以确定。作者主要还是想要表达品酒的困难性和不确定性。但是又不局限于肓品(blind tasting),因此选项B正确。
转载请注明原文地址:https://tihaiku.com/zcyy/3888846.html
相关试题推荐
______istheleaderoftheBritishgovernment.A、PrimeMinisterB、QueenC、Preside
TheAmericaneconomicsystemisorganizedaroundabasicprivate-1.______e
TheAmericaneconomicsystemisorganizedaroundabasicprivate-1.______e
TheAmericaneconomicsystemisorganizedaroundabasicprivate-1.______e
TheAmericaneconomicsystemisorganizedaroundabasicprivate-1.______e
TheAmericaneconomicsystemisorganizedaroundabasicprivate-1.______e
AhighlyorganizedsystemofirrigationistypicalwithChineseagriculture.with
OnMay24,1976,theBritishwinemerchantStevenSpurrierorganizedablind
TheexecutivepoweroftheBritishgovernmentisinthehandsof______.A、thePri
TherealandcentralpoweroftheBritishparliamentliesinthehandof______.A
随机试题
Youshouldspendabout20minutesonthistask.Thechartsbelowshowthepercen
—Wedidn’tfindtheThompsons__________thelecture.—Noonehadtoldthemabout_
Forthispart,youareallowed30minutestowriteashortessayentitledOnPay
患者女性,剧烈胸痛伴发热,ST段除aVR导联以外呈弓背向下型抬高。最可能的诊断为
左旋多巴的早期反应是A、胃肠道反应 B、运动障碍 C、症状波动 D、幻觉
船舶( )是表示船舶的营运活动应达到的要求或已达到的数量,包括运输量和生产能力
培植风险文化不是商业银行的一项“终身事业”,而是阶段性工作。(?)?
某药材,细长圆柱形。质硬脆,易折断,受潮后变软。断面黄白色,其外周散有多数黄白色
下列有关工程量计算规范和消耗量定额的叙述,正确的是()。A.消耗量定额章节划分与
最新回复
(
0
)