首页
登录
职称英语
Where does morality come from? Throughout the history of Western civilizatio
Where does morality come from? Throughout the history of Western civilizatio
游客
2024-11-29
13
管理
问题
Where does morality come from? Throughout the history of Western civilization thinkers have usually answered either that it comes from God, or else through the application of reason.
But in The Bonobo and the Atheist, primatologist Frans de Waal argues that there’s another answer that fits the data better: morality comes from our evolutionary past as a social primate. Like our closest relatives, the apes, humans evolved in small, tightly knit, cooperative groups. As a result, again like the apes, we are exquisitely sensitive to one another’s moods, needs and intentions.
This well-developed empathy provided the trellis on which morality later flowered. De Waal, who is based at Emory University in Atlanta, Georgia, has been making this case eloquently for many years and over several books, notably in Good Natured back in 1997, and in Primates and Philosophers, 12 years later.
In his new work, he bolsters the argument by drawing on a lot of new research, carefully footnoted for those who want to dig deeper. De Waal distinguishes two degrees of morality. The first he calls "one-on-one morality", which governs how an individual can expect to be treated, and the second "community concern", a larger, more abstract concept that extends to the harmony of the group as a whole.
Chimps and bonobos certainly have the former—they respect ownership, for example, and expect to be treated according to their place in the hierarchy. But de Waal presents several examples—such as a chimp stepping in to stop a fight between two others—that suggest that they also have a rudimentary form of the latter.
The book’s title, incidentally, draws on bonobos because they are more likely than chimps to behave morally, to have concern for each other, to value harmony and so on. This, imagines, de Waal, is something morally inclined atheists would want to emulate. If humans inherited morality from our ancestors, though, what are we to make of religion? Here de Waal moves into the territory he has not explored before. Clearly, religion must do something important, since every human culture has it. But instead of religion giving us morality, de Waal turns the tables. Morality, he argues, probably gave us religion as a way of reinforcing the pre-existing community concern.
If he’s right, then there may be no absolute code of right and wrong out there to be discovered. Instead, each individual’s evolved sense of empathy and concern for the group may help shape the group’s consensus on what kind of behaviour is appropriate. In short, says de Waal, morality may be something we all have to work out together. It’s a persuasive argument, and de Waal’s cautious and evidence-based approach is one that many New Scientist readers are sure to find congenial.
That careful approach is less evident in another book covering some of the same ground. In How Animals Grieve, anthropologist Barbara King sets out to explore the question of whether non-human animals grieve for their dead. It’s an intriguing question, but unfortunately King’s book is largely a succession of anecdotes: the cat who roams the house, crying, in search of its dead litter mate; the dog who waits daily at the train station for its dead master; a dolphin trying to keep her dead calf afloat for days.
Some of these stories make a persuasive case for some animals—especially apes, elephants and cetaceans— sometimes grieving. No surprises there: I suspect most readers would have conceded that ground right from the start.
But King makes little effort to dig any deeper by exploring, for example, the neural machinery and cognitive skills an animal needs in order to be capable of grief. After all, solitary species such as cats have less need for empathy- and its corollary, grief—than social animals, and small-brained creatures such as turtles may simply lack the brainpower or not form lasting pair bonds.
To his credit, de Waal takes full note of such distinctions; King, not so much. [br] Frans de Waal believes that morality comes from
选项
A、our primate ancestors.
B、social revolutions.
C、the ability of reasoning.
D、cooperative groups.
答案
A
解析
细节题。由题干中的人名和morality comes from定位到第二段第一句“But in The Bonobo and theAtheist,primatologist Frans de Waal argues that there’s another answer that fits the data better:moralitycomes from our evolutionary past as a social primate.”,可见Frans de Waal认为,作为灵长类动物,道德是由我们自身进化而来的,也就是说道德来自我们灵长类动物的祖先,故[A]为答案。
转载请注明原文地址:https://tihaiku.com/zcyy/3866528.html
相关试题推荐
Wheredoesmoralitycomefrom?ThroughoutthehistoryofWesterncivilizatio
Wheredoesmoralitycomefrom?ThroughoutthehistoryofWesterncivilizatio
Wheredoesmoralitycomefrom?ThroughoutthehistoryofWesterncivilizatio
Wheredoesmoralitycomefrom?ThroughoutthehistoryofWesterncivilizatio
______isthefirstimportantgovernessnovelintheEnglishliteraryhistory.A、J
ThefirstimmigrantsinAmericanhistorycamefrom______,somerunningawayfro
TheGrandCanyon,carvedbytheColoradoRiver,innorthwestern______isoneofn
______isapoemconcernedwiththespiritualbreakupofamoderncivilizationin
Throughoutthe1600s,______foundedpermanentsettlementsinCanada.A、Englandan
InthehistoryofGreatBritain,mostpeoplehavelivedinthe______withamilde
随机试题
Petrolismanufacturedfromthe______oilwetakeoutoftheground.A、crudeB、r
王某是一名软件设计师,每当软件开发完成后,按公司规定编写的软件文档属于职务作品,
甲公司承担了一项为宇航系统配套生产高性能嵌入式计算机系统的任务,用户要求该系统要
结合自己的工作实践,辅导员应当如何充分利用校园文化的特点发挥其育人作用?
测定某一种组织抽提物的蛋白质含量,用什么方法合适A.凯氏定氮法 B.双缩脲法
根据变电一次设备标准缺陷库,集合式电力电容器构架及基础,构架开裂,不影响闸刀设备
(2021年12月真题)期货交易内幕信息的知情人员或者非法获取期货交易内幕信息的
参与DNA复制的物质不包括A.DNA聚合酶 B.解链酶、拓扑酶 C.模板、引
(2019年真题)某企业当年实际销售费用为6000万元,占销售额的30%,
A.放疗 B.化疗十放疗 C.化疗 D.电化学治疗 E.手术男性,50岁
最新回复
(
0
)