首页
登录
职称英语
When the Viaduct de Millau opened in the south of France in 2004, this talle
When the Viaduct de Millau opened in the south of France in 2004, this talle
游客
2024-11-18
15
管理
问题
When the Viaduct de Millau opened in the south of France in 2004, this tallest bridge in the world won worldwide compliments. German newspapers described how it "floated above the clouds" with "elegance and lightness" and "breathtaking" beauty. In France, papers praised the "immense concrete giant". Was it mere coincidence that the Germans saw beauty where the French saw heft and power? Lera Boroditsky thinks not.
A psychologist at Stanford University, she has long been intrigued by an age-old question whose modern form dates to 1956, when linguist Benjamin Lee Whorf asked whether the language we speak shapes the way we think and see the world. If so, then language is not merely a means of expressing thought, but a constraint on it, too. Although philosophers, anthropologists, and others have weighed in, with most concluding that language does not shape thought in any significant way, the field has been notable for a distressing lack of empiricism—as in testable hypotheses and actual data.
That’s where Boroditsky comes in. In a series of clever experiments guided by pointed questions, she is amassing evidence that, yes, language shapes thought. The effect is powerful enough, she says, that "the private mental lives of speakers of different languages may differ dramatically," not only when they are thinking in order to speak, "but in all manner of cognitive tasks," including basic sensory perception. "Even a small fluke of grammar"—the gender of nouns—"can have an effect on how people think about things in the world," she says.
As in that bridge, in German, the noun for bridge, Brucke, is feminine. In French, pont is masculine. German speakers saw female features; French speakers, masculine ones. Similarly, Germans describe keys (Schlussel) with words such as hard, heavy, jagged, and metal, while to Spaniards keys (Haves) are golden, intricate, little, and lovely. Guess which language interprets key as masculine and which as feminine?
Language even shapes what we see. People have a better memory for colors if different shades have distinct names—not English’s light blue and dark blue, for instance, but Russian’s goluboy and sinly. Skeptics of the language-shapes-thought claim have argued that that’s a trivial finding, showing only that people remember what they saw in both a visual form and a verbal one, but not proving that they actually see the hues differently. In an ingenious experiment, however, Boroditsky and colleagues showed volunteers three color swatches and asked them which of the bottom two was the same as the top one. Native Russian speakers were faster than English speakers when the colors had distinct names, suggesting that having a name for something allows you to perceive it more sharply. Similarly, Korean uses one word for "in" when one object is in another snugly (a letter in an envelope), and a different one when an object is in something loosely (an apple in a bowl). Sure enough, Korean adults are better than English speakers at distinguishing tight fit from loose fit.
In Australia, the Aboriginal Kuuk Thaayorre use compass directions for every spatial cue rather than right or left, leading to locutions such as "there is an ant on your southeast leg. " The Kuuk Thaayorre are also much more skillful than English speakers at dead reckoning, even in unfamiliar surroundings or strange buildings. Their language "equips them to perform navigational feats once thought beyond human capabilities," Boroditsky wrote on Edge.org.
Science has only scratched the surface of how language affects thought. In Russian, verb forms indicate whether the action was completed or not—as in "she ate (and finished)the pizza." In Turkish, verbs indicate whether the action was observed or merely rumored. Boroditsky would love to run an experiment testing whether native Russian speakers are better than others at noticing if an action is completed, and if Turks have a heightened sensitivity to fact versus hearsay. Similarly, while English says "she broke the bowl," even if it smashed accidentally (she dropped something on it, say), Spanish and Japanese describe the same event more like "the bowl broke itself. "" When we show people video of the same event," says Boroditsky, " English speakers remember who was to blame even in an accident, but Spanish and Japanese speakers remember it less well than they do intentional actions. It raises questions about whether language affects even something as basic as how we construct our ideas of causality. " [br] In the first paragraph, the author introduces his topic by________.
选项
A、explaining a phenomenon
B、justifying an assumption
C、posing a contrast
D、making a comparison
答案
C
解析
细节题。第一段中,作者拿德国媒体和法国媒体对密约高架桥不同的描述方法来进行对比,引出下文中他想提出的问题:语言是否决定思维。[A]“解释一个现象”、[B]“证明一种假设”、[D]“做出类比”都不符合第一段的内容,只有[C]“进行对比”正确。
转载请注明原文地址:https://tihaiku.com/zcyy/3851605.html
相关试题推荐
WhentheViaductdeMillauopenedinthesouthofFrancein2004,thistalle
WhentheViaductdeMillauopenedinthesouthofFrancein2004,thistalle
PassageFour[br]AccordingtoFrancesO’Grady,what’sthestatusofmodernecon
HighinthemountainsofsouthernFrance,thesleepytownofAurillachasfe
FranceintheTwentiethCenturyI.FranceinWorldWarI1914:Germanydecla
FranceintheTwentiethCenturyI.FranceinWorldWarI1914:Germanydecla
FranceintheTwentiethCenturyI.FranceinWorldWarI1914:Germanydecla
FranceintheTwentiethCenturyI.FranceinWorldWarI1914:Germanydecla
FranceintheTwentiethCenturyI.FranceinWorldWarI1914:Germanydecla
FranceintheTwentiethCenturyI.FranceinWorldWarI1914:Germanydecla
随机试题
BilingualEducationBilingualeducationprov
简述组织、领导、参加恐怖组织罪的构成要件。
A.生后2~3天至2个月 B.2个月 C.8个月 D.3个月以上 E.1
新生儿期的特点是A.语言、思维和交往能力增强,但对各种危险的识别能力不足 B.
患儿女,2岁半。因发热3小时来院,在门诊就诊时出现惊厥发作,送入急诊室时发作仍未
2020年12月,C市天然气用量为9.67亿立方米,同比增长11.66%。从供应
根据保险法等法律、法规的有关规定,保险合同的生效要件有()。A.主体必须合格
在玻璃幕墙施工过程中,一般不能直接将玻璃坐落在金属框上,须在金属框内垫上一层(
(2020年真题)《根据建设工程造价鉴定规范》下列现场签证争议鉴定的做法,正确的
人际传播的特点不包括A.信息量大 B.传播较慢 C.简便易行 D.针对性高
最新回复
(
0
)