首页
登录
职称英语
Should A Kid Be Guided to Tell a White Lie? It’s my family
Should A Kid Be Guided to Tell a White Lie? It’s my family
游客
2024-02-26
16
管理
问题
Should A Kid Be Guided to Tell a White Lie?
It’s my family’s tradition to exchange girls on Christmas Eve. Before we did so, I whispered to my uncle and his wife, "Just want you to know: I think what I got you is really cool, so just tell me you like it, no matter what, okay?"
I know that sounds rude, but there’s another Christmas custom in my family: we give each other weird gifts.
There is a sweet reason for this. My grandparents grew up during the Depression, and there were years when they had no gifts at all. So my grandmother and her siblings(兄弟姐妹) would gift-wrap their old socks and clothes, just so they had something to open on Christmas. Pretend presents were better than none at all.
My grandmother never really got over those early years, so, for the rest of her life, she went a little crazy at the holidays. She’d start buying gifts in October. It didn’t matter what it was. Socks, toothbrushes, used paperbacks she’d read but didn’t like, all went under the tree. Contents of catalog "mystery boxes" meant we spent another hour unwrapping presents. One of my more memorable gifts: a single piece of clear plastic labeled "face shield." I was apparently to hold it in front of my eyes when I used hair spray.
We all thanked Grandma greatly no matter what we got. As a little kid, this pattern of gratitude for the terrible presents puzzled me; it took a long time for me to understand it was all right to laugh at some of her gifts. Now I don’t really know if my aunt and uncle actually liked the gift I gave them. They said they did, but since I coached them to tell me they love it, I’ll never really know if that was the truth.
All of these make me think of the work of McGill professor Victoria Talwar. As an expert in children’s lying behavior, Talwar has been studying how kids respond to unwanted gifts. When they get a gift they hate, can they still thank someone and pretend to love it?
Talwar tests kids’ ability to do this, by asking kids to pick a toy they want; if they win a game, they get the chosen toy. There are plastic horses, a small car, a few other items, including an unwrapped, dirty, worn, used bar of soap. At some point in the game, there’s a switch in the adults who play with the kids. So, instead of giving the child her chosen toy, the late-arriving adult gives the child the soap.
Then, the researchers watch what happens. 68% of kids, aged 3 to 11, will spontaneously say they love the gift of old ugly soap. The older they are, the more likely they are to say a white lie about the gift. And if parents encourage the children to say how much they like the present, the percentage of kids lying about the gift increases to 87%.
At this point, some may be saying that a white lie isn’t a lie. That’s because you are looking at lying from the adult perspective--that lies are acceptable, when told with the intent of helping someone, or protecting another’s feelings. But kids don’t think of lying in the same way. For them, the intent behind a lie--for good or for ill--is irrelevant. It is so irrelevant that, for very young kids, you can’t even lie by accident. Someone who gives out wrong information, but believed it to be true, is still a liar in these kids’ book.
Kids just don’t believe that lying comes in shades of white or gray. Lying is much simpler than that: lying is telling somebody something that isn’t so; lying is really bad; and lying gets you punished. And if it gets you punished, you shouldn’t do it. In Talwar’s lab, parents have literally cheered to hear their kids lie about how great it is to have received the old soap. The parents have pride over their children’s knowing the socially appropriate response.
Talwar’s regularly amazed by this. The parents never even seem to realize that the child told a lie. They never want to scold the child afterwards, or talk about the kid’s behavior.
Regardless of the parents’ pride, the kids aren’t happy about their successfully lying. Instead, it can be torture for them.
I was at Talwar’s lab when she was doing a version of the unwanted gift experiment with kids in the first and second grades. Watching kid after kid react to that gross bar of soap, I could really see how emotionally difficult it is for kids to tell a white lie. The kids were disappointed when they were handed the soap, but that was nothing compared to the discomfort they showed while having to lie about liking it. They are uneasy. Some looked like they were going to cry. It was simply painful to watch. Indeed, Talwar has found that some kids just can’t even bring themselves to say something nice about the present. About 20% of 11-year-olds just refuse to tell a white lie about that unwanted gift--even after their parents encouraged them to do so. And about 14% of kids still won’t tell a white lie, even after their parents specifically explained the prosocial (亲社会的) reasons to tell the lie. These kids just can’t reconcile the disconnect between knowing how bad lying is, and being told they should now lie. Talwar cautions that we need to recognize that, at least from the kids’ point of view, white lies really are still lies. We should take care to explain the motivation behind the untruth--that we want to protect the other person’s feelings. Kids may still fail to completely understand the distinction, but at least it will encourage them to think about others’ feelings when they act. And we need to reassure children that they won’t be punished for a specific white lie--because they did something nice for someone else.
Talwar also warns that we adults should pay attention to our own use of white lies. Kids notice these untruths-and that we rarely get punished for them. If kids believe that we regularly lie to get out of uncomfortable social situations, they are more likely to adopt a similar strategy of lying. If we don’t watch it, we could unintentionally be giving kids yet another present: a license to lie. [br] When the kids are telling lies about the ugly soap, they feel ______.
选项
A、disappointed
B、indifferent
C、uncomfortable
D、satisfied
答案
C
解析
该段第3句说到孩子说谎时表现出来的是discomfort,第4句又用uneasy补充说明孩子们说谎时的感觉,uncomfortable与这两个意思相近,故选C。A为强干扰项,disappointed是孩子们收到不喜欢的香皂时表露出来的情绪,不是他们说谎时的感觉,故排除A。此题题干的定位依据词不明显,也可根据第6题和第8题的定位来确定答题依据处,通常出题点是按原文的顺序来出题,此题定位处可在这两题之间查找。
转载请注明原文地址:https://tihaiku.com/zcyy/3483494.html
相关试题推荐
Theywouldn’tgiveuptryingevenif______(他们再次失败).theyshouldfailonceagain.
Wehavespokenofmarriageasaformalcontract.Itshouldbenoted,however
Wehavespokenofmarriageasaformalcontract.Itshouldbenoted,however
Wehavespokenofmarriageasaformalcontract.Itshouldbenoted,however
Wehavespokenofmarriageasaformalcontract.Itshouldbenoted,however
Wehavespokenofmarriageasaformalcontract.Itshouldbenoted,however
Wehavespokenofmarriageasaformalcontract.Itshouldbenoted,however
Wehavespokenofmarriageasaformalcontract.Itshouldbenoted,however
Wehavespokenofmarriageasaformalcontract.Itshouldbenoted,however
Wehavespokenofmarriageasaformalcontract.Itshouldbenoted,however
随机试题
Itisanticipatedthatbytheendofthismonthoilprice_______byabout10%.A
高跷原是宋代“百戏”中的一种舞蹈样式。()
关于管涵施工技术要点的说法,错误的有()。A、管涵通常采用工厂预制钢筋混凝土管的
分部分项工程成本分析的资料来源是()A、投标报价成本、施工概算、实际工程量、实际
如右图所示,某条河流一侧有A、B两家工厂,与河岸的距离分别为4km和5km,且A
催产素利于胎儿娩出是由于其A.与剂量无关 B.兴奋宫颈作用大于宫体作用 C.
将下列选项中的词语依次填入各句横线处,最恰当的一项是: (1)春运是我国特别的
罗西(A.Rossi)城市空间类型学中的类型要素不包括( )。A.城市街道
现场总线控制系统的特点有()。A.现场总线为开放式的互联网络 B.不同生产厂
土壤砷的卫生标准为15mg/kg,某地区土壤砷环境本底值为8.7mg/kg,则本
最新回复
(
0
)