首页
登录
职称英语
By now, it should come as no surprise when scientists discover yet another c
By now, it should come as no surprise when scientists discover yet another c
游客
2023-12-14
36
管理
问题
By now, it should come as no surprise when scientists discover yet another case of experience changing the brain. From the sensory information we absorb to the movements we make, our lives leave footprints on the bumps and fissures of our cortex, so much so that experiences can alter "hard-wired" brain structures. Through rehab, stroke patients can coax a region of the motor cortex on the opposite side of the damaged region to pinch-hit, restoring lost mobility; volunteers who are blindfolded for just five days can reprogram their visual cortex to process sound and touch.
Still, scientists have been surprised at how deeply culture—the language we speak, the values we absorb—shapes the brain, and are rethinking findings derived from studies of Westerners. To take one recent example, a region behind the forehead called the medial prefrontal cortex supposedly represents the self: it is active when we ("we" being the Americans in the study) think of our own identity and traits. But with Chinese volunteers, the results were strikingly different. The "me" circuit hummed not only when they thought whether a particular adjective described themselves, but also when they considered whether it described their mother. The Westerners showed no such overlap between self and mom. Depending whether one lives in a culture that views the self as autonomous and unique or as connected to and part of a larger whole, this neural circuit takes on quite different functions.
"Cultural neuroscience," as this new field is called, is about discovering such differences. Some of the findings, as with the "me/mom" circuit, buttress longstanding notions of cultural differences. For instance, it is a cultural cliché that Westerners focus on individual objects while East Asians pay attention to context and background (another manifestation of the individualism-collectivism split). Sure enough, when shown complex, busy scenes, Asian-Americans and non-Asian—Americans recruited different brain regions. The Asians showed more activity in areas that process figure-ground relations—holistic context—while the Americans showed more activity in regions that recognize objects.
Psychologist Nalini Ambady of Tufts found something similar when she and colleagues showed drawings of people in a submissive pose (head down, shoulders hunched) or a dominant one (arms crossed, face forward) to Japanese and Americans. The brain’s dopamine-fueled reward circuit became most active at the sight of the stance—dominant for Americans, submissive for Japanese—that each volunteer’s culture most values, they reported in 2009. This raises an obvious chicken-and-egg question.
Cultural neuroscience wouldn’t be making waves if it found neurobiological bases only for well-known cultural differences. It is also uncovering the unexpected. For instance, a 2006 study found that native Chinese speakers use a different region of the brain to do simple arithmetic (3+ 4) or decide which number is larger than native English speakers do, even though both use Arabic numerals. The Chinese use the circuits that process visual and spatial information and plan movements (the latter may be related to the use of the abacus). But English speakers use language circuits. It is as if the West Conceives numbers as just words, but the East imbues them with symbolic, spatial freight. "One would think that neural processes involving basic mathematical computations are universal," says Ambady, but they "seem to be culture-specific. "
Not to be the skunk at this party, but I think it’s important to ask whether neuroscience reveals anything more than we already know from, say, anthropology. For instance, it’s well known that East Asian cultures prize the collective over the individual, and that Americans do the opposite.
Ambady thinks cultural neuroscience does advance understanding. Take the me/mom finding, which, she argues, "attests to the strength of the overlap between self and people close to you in collectivistic cultures and the separation in individualistic cultures. It is important to push the analysis to the level of the brain. " Especially when it shows how fundamental cultural differences are—so fundamental, perhaps, that "universal" notions such as human rights, democracy, and the like may be no such thing. [br] What does "buttress" mean in the third paragraph?
选项
A、contradict
B、doubt
C、are unrelated to
D、support
答案
D
解析
原文第二段中the“me/mom”circuit的发现,表明文化背景和价值观会改变大脑结构。所以第三段中的“buttress”应该指“支持、强化”之意。
转载请注明原文地址:https://tihaiku.com/zcyy/3272617.html
相关试题推荐
[originaltext]M:Thoughshouldchocolateloverseverywhereberejoicingtoday,
[originaltext]M:Thoughshouldchocolateloverseverywhereberejoicingtoday,
[originaltext]M:Thoughshouldchocolateloverseverywhereberejoicingtoday,
IftheFederationofAmericanScientistsmadealistofeducationalvideogame
[originaltext]InterviewwithPAULRAY:AD:HowdidyoudiscovertheCulturalCr
[originaltext]InterviewwithPAULRAY:AD:HowdidyoudiscovertheCulturalCr
[originaltext]InterviewwithPAULRAY:AD:HowdidyoudiscovertheCulturalCr
JonasFrisenhad’hiseurekamomentin1997.Backthen,scientistssuspectedt
JonasFrisenhad’hiseurekamomentin1997.Backthen,scientistssuspectedt
JonasFrisenhad’hiseurekamomentin1997.Backthen,scientistssuspectedt
随机试题
ClassicIntentionMovementInsocialsituations,thecl
StructureoftheCanadianGovernmentI.IntroductionA.Ca
word界面中,由()部分组成。A.快速访问工具栏 B.打印区 C.编
某项目经理正在为一个新产品开发项目制订项目管理计划,他应该遵循的基本原则中不包括
血证出血部位与脏腑的关系下列哪项是错误的A.咯血、鼻衄多与肺有关 B.肌衄多与
根据下列资料回答问题。 2010年,我国进出口贸易总额为29727.6亿美元
《中国教育改革和发展纲要》提出了教育发展的“两基”“两全”“两重”目标,其中“两
下列关于《行政处罚法》的说法,正确的是( )。A.罚款不可以折抵罚金 B.受
货币政策的类型有( )。A.稳健型货币政策 B.均衡性货币政策 C.扩张性
党的十八大报告进一步充实和完善了我国就业的方针,第一次将()纳入就业方针。
最新回复
(
0
)