首页
登录
职称英语
The Happiness Effect The next time you get the flu,
The Happiness Effect The next time you get the flu,
游客
2023-08-29
20
管理
问题
The Happiness Effect
The next time you get the flu, there will almost certainly be someone you can blame for your pain. There’s the inconsiderate co-worker who decided to drag himself to the office and spent the day sniffling, sneezing and shivering in the cubicle next to yours. Or your child’s best friend, the one who showed up for a playdate with a runny nose and a short supply of tissues. Then there’s the guy at the gym who spent more time sneezing than sweating on the treadmill before you used it.
You’re right to pass the blame. Pathogens(致病菌)like the influenza virus pass like a holiday fruitcake from person to person, but you probably don’t think much past the one who gave it directly to you. An infectious-disease expert, on the other hand, would not be satisfied to stop there. What about the person who passed the virus on to your colleague, the one before him and others earlier still? Contagious(传染性的)diseases operate like a giant infectious network, spreading like the latest YouTube clip a-mong friends of friends online. We’re social animals; we share.
So public-health experts are beginning to wonder whether certain health-related behaviors are just as contagious as microbes. If you’re struggling with your weight, did you in effect catch a case of fat by learning poor eating and exercise habits from a friend or family member who was similarly infected by someone else? If you smoke, do you light up because you were behaviorally contaminated by smokers who convinced you of the coolness of the habit? Even more important, if such unhealthy behaviors are contagious, are healthy ones — like quitting smoking or exercising — equally so? And what if not only behaviors but also moods and mental states work the same way? Can you catch a case of happy?
Increasingly, the answer seems to be yes. That’s the intriguing conclusion from a body of work by Harvard social scientist Dr. Nicholas Christakis and his political-science colleague James Fowler at the University of California at San Diego. The pair created a sensation with their announcement earlier this month of a 20-year study showing that emotions can pass among a network of people up to three degrees of separation away, so your joy may, to a larger extent than you realize, be determined by how cheerful your friends’ friends’ friends are, even if some of the people in this chain are total strangers to you.
If that’s so, it creates a whole new paradigm for the way people get sick and, more important, how to get them healthy. It may mean that an individual’s well-being is the product not just of his behaviors and emotions but more of the way they feed into a larger social network. Think of it as health Facebook-style. "We have a collective identity as a population that transcends individual identity, " says Christakis. "This superorgan-ism has an anatomy(解剖学), physiology, structure and function that we are trying to understand."
In their most recent paper, published in the British Medical Journal, Christakis and Fowler explored the emotional state of nearly 5,000 people and the more than 50, 000 social ties they shared. At three points during the long study, all the participants answered a standard questionnaire to determine their happiness level, so that the scientists could track changes in emotional state. That led to their intriguing finding of just how contagious happiness can be: if a subject’s friend was happy, that subject was 15% more likely to be happy too; if that friend’s friend was happy, the original subject was 10% more likely to be so. Even if the subject’s friend’s friend’s friend — entirely unknown to the subject — was happy, the subject still got a 5.6% boost. The happiness chain also worked in the other direction, radiating from the subject out to her friends.
The happiness dividend is more powerful if two people not only know each other but also are equally fond of each other. Happiness is more infectious in mutual relationships(in which both people name the other as a friend)than in unreciprocated ones(in which only one is named).
And it’s not just in sterile(无生气的)study settings that the contagion of happiness is spreading. Christakis and Fowler noticed that people who are smiling on their Facebook pages tend to cluster together, forming an online social circle like a delirious flock of cyberbirds. And while some of this joy can certainly be traced to the copycat effect — if your friends post smiling pictures, you might feel like a grouch(不高兴的人)if you don’t too — Christakis and Fowler are analyzing the clusters to see if something more infectious might be at work.
Skeptics raise other concerns, ones that go beyond the copycat effect. Couldn’t happy people simply be exposed to similar lifestyles or social factors that explain their shared joy, such as favorable weather, low unemployment rates or a winning baseball team? If that were the case, argue the authors, then happiness would spread more uniformly among all the relationships; instead, it varied depending on whether the friendship was mutual or merely one-sided. As the investigators teased out these factors, they found that environment didn’t have nearly the power that relationships did.
The infectiousness of happiness is only the latest in a series of similar phenomena Christakis and Fowler have studied. In 2007 they published a paper showing that obesity travels across webs in a similar way, with individuals having a 57% greater risk of being overweight if they have an obese friend. The same holds true for quitting smoking, with success 30% more common among friends of quitters than among friends of smokers.
In all these cases, there’s a predictable topography(地形)to how people influence one another, one that can be reduced to a sort of social map. People who are central to their networks — who in effect are the hub through which most of the other relationships or information flows — may have the most influence on others and in turn are the most influenced by them. But just because you start off at the center of your web does not guarantee that you’ll stay there. In the 1970s, smokers were more likely to occupy that focal position in their network of friends and family. Look at a similar social map today, and you’ll see that the smokers have drifted to the periphery(外围).
The better this kind of mapping becomes, the more value it has. Officials at the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention(CDC)are exploiting the connectedness of youngsters in online social networks, for example, to improve flu-vaccination rates, not just among those under age 18 but among all the people to whom these children have ties. "Because of their social and peer networks, children have a higher likelihood of sharing information with the most people, "says Jay Bernhardt of the CDC. By targeting youngsters on these sites with information about the importance of annual flu shots, health officials hope to trigger a literal and figurative viral wave of vaccination among the kids’ peers, their peers’ peers, and even those peers’ parents and grandparents.
"We are always looking for exciting new areas of research that will help people live healthier," says Richard Suzman, director of the division of behavioral and social research at the National Institute on Aging. "Without a doubt, I see this as a very promising area. " And with the health community a web like any other, expect that idea to spread further and further. [br] Why couldn’t environment factors explain people’s shared joy?
选项
A、Because the spreading of happiness varied among different relationships.
B、Because happiness spread uniformly in different environment.
C、Because patterns of friendship decided the varying of happiness spreading.
D、Because environment factors didn’t have the power to affect happiness.
答案
A
解析
第三句说人们只是因为环境因素改变情绪的话,那么在不同的人际关系中,快乐的传播方式应该很单一,但事实并不如此,因双方和单方友谊关系的不同,快乐的传播也随之不同,由此可知环境因素不能单独解释人们共享快乐的原因是[A]。原文没有提到快乐在不同的环境中以同种方式传播,故[B]项错误。原文只是说环境没有人际关系那么大影响力,但不是说快乐的传播方式只决定于友谊关系,环境没有任何影响力,故[C][D]两项错误。
转载请注明原文地址:https://tihaiku.com/zcyy/2967239.html
相关试题推荐
【B1】[br]【B8】[originaltext]Shynessisthecauseofmuchunhappinessforag
【B1】[br]【B6】[originaltext]Shynessisthecauseofmuchunhappinessforag
【B1】[br]【B4】[originaltext]Shynessisthecauseofmuchunhappinessforag
【B1】[br]【B3】[originaltext]Shynessisthecauseofmuchunhappinessforag
【B1】[br]【B2】[originaltext]Shynessisthecauseofmuchunhappinessforag
Shynessisthecauseofmuchunhappinessforagreatmanypeople.Shypeople
【B1】[br]【B20】A、effectsB、tabletsC、acidsD、chemicalsC语意辨析。由上一句的内容可知C为最佳答案。
【S1】[br]【S18】A、regardedB、impressedC、influencedD、effectedC语义衔接题。注意空格处动作的主语为
【S1】[br]【S8】A、conceptB、dimensionC、effectD、perspectiveD惯用衔接题。所填词需与in搭配,由此首先
Antiseptics(杀菌剂)havesavedcountlesslives,buttheyaremosteffectivewh
随机试题
Thedrinkwaspackagedinchampagnebottlesandwasbeing______astherealstu
Ofallhisnovels,Ilikethisone(well)______.best在他所有的小说中,我最喜欢这一本。在本句中,因为前面有介
PartⅡReadingComprehension(SkimmingandScanning)Directions:Inthispart,y
TVLinkedtoLowerMarksA)Theeffectoftelevisiononc
患者女性52岁,缺失,行固定义齿修复.46为基牙。1年后基牙.有跳痛。其主要原因
光缆布线信道应采用多模光纤和单模光纤。()
卡托普利的作用靶点是A.血管紧张素转化酶B.β-肾上腺素受体C.羟甲戊二酰辅酶A
据报道,现在有些医院已采取了一些隔离措施,使体格检查置于一个相对封闭的环境中,以
以下关于Excel数据清单的说法中正确的有()。A、用Excel的数据清单查找记
(2019年真题)硬聚氯乙烯给水管应用较广泛,下列关于此管表述正确的为()
最新回复
(
0
)