首页
登录
职称英语
Where does morality come from? Throughout the history of Western civilizatio
Where does morality come from? Throughout the history of Western civilizatio
游客
2024-11-29
30
管理
问题
Where does morality come from? Throughout the history of Western civilization thinkers have usually answered either that it comes from God, or else through the application of reason.
But in The Bonobo and the Atheist, primatologist Frans de Waal argues that there’s another answer that fits the data better: morality comes from our evolutionary past as a social primate. Like our closest relatives, the apes, humans evolved in small, tightly knit, cooperative groups. As a result, again like the apes, we are exquisitely sensitive to one another’s moods, needs and intentions.
This well-developed empathy provided the trellis on which morality later flowered. De Waal, who is based at Emory University in Atlanta, Georgia, has been making this case eloquently for many years and over several books, notably in Good Natured back in 1997, and in Primates and Philosophers, 12 years later.
In his new work, he bolsters the argument by drawing on a lot of new research, carefully footnoted for those who want to dig deeper. De Waal distinguishes two degrees of morality. The first he calls "one-on-one morality", which governs how an individual can expect to be treated, and the second "community concern", a larger, more abstract concept that extends to the harmony of the group as a whole.
Chimps and bonobos certainly have the former—they respect ownership, for example, and expect to be treated according to their place in the hierarchy. But de Waal presents several examples—such as a chimp stepping in to stop a fight between two others—that suggest that they also have a rudimentary form of the latter.
The book’s title, incidentally, draws on bonobos because they are more likely than chimps to behave morally, to have concern for each other, to value harmony and so on. This, imagines, de Waal, is something morally inclined atheists would want to emulate. If humans inherited morality from our ancestors, though, what are we to make of religion? Here de Waal moves into the territory he has not explored before. Clearly, religion must do something important, since every human culture has it. But instead of religion giving us morality, de Waal turns the tables. Morality, he argues, probably gave us religion as a way of reinforcing the pre-existing community concern.
If he’s right, then there may be no absolute code of right and wrong out there to be discovered. Instead, each individual’s evolved sense of empathy and concern for the group may help shape the group’s consensus on what kind of behaviour is appropriate. In short, says de Waal, morality may be something we all have to work out together. It’s a persuasive argument, and de Waal’s cautious and evidence-based approach is one that many New Scientist readers are sure to find congenial.
That careful approach is less evident in another book covering some of the same ground. In How Animals Grieve, anthropologist Barbara King sets out to explore the question of whether non-human animals grieve for their dead. It’s an intriguing question, but unfortunately King’s book is largely a succession of anecdotes: the cat who roams the house, crying, in search of its dead litter mate; the dog who waits daily at the train station for its dead master; a dolphin trying to keep her dead calf afloat for days.
Some of these stories make a persuasive case for some animals—especially apes, elephants and cetaceans— sometimes grieving. No surprises there: I suspect most readers would have conceded that ground right from the start.
But King makes little effort to dig any deeper by exploring, for example, the neural machinery and cognitive skills an animal needs in order to be capable of grief. After all, solitary species such as cats have less need for empathy- and its corollary, grief—than social animals, and small-brained creatures such as turtles may simply lack the brainpower or not form lasting pair bonds.
To his credit, de Waal takes full note of such distinctions; King, not so much. [br] The author find Barbara King’s book
选项
A、persuasive.
B、surprising.
C、suspicious.
D、unconvincing.
答案
D
解析
推断题。由题干中的Barbara King’s book定位到第九、十段。第九段最后一句提到,Barbara King的书写的主要是一系列轶事,此外,第十段最后一句作者明确说他认为大多数读者会承认这本书从开始论述的就是一个既定的事实,总结这两处不难看出,作者认为Barbara King的书是没有说服力的,因此选择[D]。
转载请注明原文地址:http://tihaiku.com/zcyy/3866531.html
相关试题推荐
Wheredoesmoralitycomefrom?ThroughoutthehistoryofWesterncivilizatio
Wheredoesmoralitycomefrom?ThroughoutthehistoryofWesterncivilizatio
Wheredoesmoralitycomefrom?ThroughoutthehistoryofWesterncivilizatio
TheGrandCanyon,carvedbytheColoradoRiver,innorthwestern______isoneofn
______isapoemconcernedwiththespiritualbreakupofamoderncivilizationin
______isNOTwrittenbyWashingtonIrving.A、AHistoryofNewYorkB、TheLegendo
Throughoutthe1600s,______foundedpermanentsettlementsinCanada.A、Englandan
ThefirstimmigrantsinAmericanhistorycamefrom______.A、EnglandB、theNetherl
Itisin______thatTrumanconvincedtheWesternEuropeanpowerstojointheNort
FrancisBacon’s______isregardedasamilestoneinthehistoryofEnglishprose
随机试题
[originaltext]M:Susan,doyouknowhowlongittakestoapplyforavisaforC
[originaltext]Americanwomenareusedtobeingindependent.Theyareusedt
基地内建筑面积大于3000m2且只有一条环通的基地道路与城市道路相连接时,基地道
帕金森病的首发症状常见的是A.静止性震颤 B.肌强直 C.运动迟缓 D.随
麦冬中柱维管束是A.周木型B.双韧型C.外韧型D.辐射型E.周韧型
关于OBOS,下列表述正确的是()。A:形态理论和切线理论中的结论也可用于OBO
计算机能直接执行的程序是()。 A.源程序 B.目标程序 C.汇编程序
按贷款用途划分,公司信贷的种类不包括()。A:自营贷款 B:并购贷款 C:
下列文件中,属于设计施工总承包合同组成文件而不属于标准施工合同组成文件的是(
不属于妊娠早期保健应注意的事项是A.及早进行第一次产前检查 B.及早确诊妊娠
最新回复
(
0
)