首页
登录
职称英语
Science of setbacks : How failure can impr
Science of setbacks : How failure can impr
游客
2024-01-21
38
管理
问题
Science of setbacks : How failure can improve career prospects
A) How do early career setbacks affect our long-term success? Failures can help us learn and overcome our fears. But disasters can still wound us. They can screw us up and set us back. Wouldn’t it be nice if there was genuine, scientifically documented truth to the expression "what doesn’t kill you makes you stronger" ?
B) One way social scientists have probed the effects of career setbacks is to look at scientists of very similar qualifications. These scientists, for reasons that are mostly arbitrary, either just missed getting a research grant or just barely made it. In social sciences, this is known as examining "near misses" and "narrow wins" in areas where merit is subjective. That allows researchers to measure only the effects of being chosen or not. Studies in this area have found conflicting results. In the competitive game of biomedical science, research has been done on scientists who narrowly lost or won grant money. It suggests that narrow winners become even bigger winners down the line. In other words, the rich get richer.
C) A 2018 study published in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, for example, followed researchers in the Netherlands. Researchers concluded that those who just barely qualified for a grant were able to get twice as much money within the next eight years as those who just missed out. And the narrow winners were 50 percent more likely to be given a professorship.
D) Others in the US have found similar effects with National Institutes of Health early-career fellowships launching narrow winners far ahead of close losers. The phenomenon is often referred to as the Matthew effect, inspired by the Bible’s wisdom that to those who have, more will be given. There’s a good explanation for the phenomenon in the book The Formula: The Universal Laws of Success by Albert Laszlo Barabasi. According to Barabasi, it’s easier and less risky for those in positions of power to choose to hand awards and funding to those who’ve already been so recognized.
E) This is bad news for the losers. Small early career setbacks seem to have a disproportionate effect down the line. What didn’t kill them made them weaker. But other studies using the same technique have shown there’s sometimes no penalty to a near miss. Students who just miss getting into top high schools or universities do just as well later in life as those who just manage to get accepted. In this case, what didn’t kill them simply didn’t matter. So is there any evidence that setbacks might actually improve our career prospects? There is now.
F) In a study published in Nature Communications, Northwestern University sociologist Dashun Wang tracked more than 1,100 scientists who were on the border between getting a grant and missing out between 1990 and 2005. He followed various measures of performance over the next decade. These included how many papers they authored and how influential those papers were, as measured by the number of subsequent citations. As expected, there was a much higher rate of attrition (减员) among scientists who didn’t get grants. But among those who stayed on, the close losers performed even better than the narrow winners. To make sure this wasn’t by chance, Wang conducted additional tests using different performance measures. He examined how many times people were first authors on influential studies, and the like.
G) One straightforward reason close losers might outperform narrow winners is that the two groups have comparable ability. In Wang’s study, he selected the most determined, passionate scientists from the loser group and culled (剔除) what he deemed the weakest members of the winner group. Yet the persevering losers still came out on top. He thinks that being a close loser might give people a psychological boost, or the proverbial kick in the pants.
H) Utrecht University sociologist Arnout van de Rijt was the lead author on the 2018 paper showing the rich get richer. He said the new finding is apparently reasonable and worth some attention. His own work showed that although the narrow winners did get much more money in the near future, the actual performance of the close losers was just as good.
I) He said the people who should be paying regard to the Wang paper are the funding agents who distribute government grant money. After all, by continuing to pile riches on the narrow winners, the taxpayers are not getting the maximum bang for their buck if the close losers are performing just as well or even better. There’s a huge amount of time and effort that goes into the process of selecting who gets grants, he said, and the latest research shows that the scientific establishment is not very good at distributing money. "Maybe we should spend less money trying to figure out who is better than who,"he said, suggesting that some more equal dividing up of money might be more productive and more efficient. Van de Rijt said he’s not convinced that losing out gives people a psychological boost. It may yet be a selection effect. Even though Wang tried to account for this by culling the weakest winners, it’s impossible to know which of the winners would have quit had they found themselves on the losing side.
J) For his part, Wang said that in his own experience, losing did light a motivating fire. He recalled a recent paper he submitted to a journal, which accepted it only to request extensive editing, and then reversed course and rejected it. He submitted the unedited version to a more respected journal and got accepted.
K) In sports and many areas of life, we think of failures as evidence of something we could have done better. We regard these disappointments as a fate we could have avoided with more careful preparation, different training, a better strategy, or more focus. And there it makes sense that failures show us the road to success. These papers deal with a kind of failure people have little control over—rejection. Others determine who wins and who loses. But at the very least, the research is starting to show that early setbacks don’t have to be fatal. They might even make us better at our jobs. Getting paid like a winner, though? That’s a different matter. [br] One researcher suggests it might be more fruitful to distribute grants on a relatively equal basis.
选项
答案
I
解析
定位句指出,也许我们应该花更少的钱试着来弄清楚谁比谁更优秀,这暗示一些更平等的资金分配可能会更有成效,更有效率。题干中的more fruitful和on a relatively equal basis分别对应定位句中的more productive and more efficient和more equal,而题干中的distribute grants是对定位句中的dividing up of money的同义转述,故答案为I)。
转载请注明原文地址:http://tihaiku.com/zcyy/3382662.html
相关试题推荐
Averyinterestingandimportantdevelopmentinscienceisadevicecalledl
Averyinterestingandimportantdevelopmentinscienceisadevicecalledl
Scienceofsetbacks:Howfailurecanimpr
Scienceofsetbacks:Howfailurecanimpr
Scienceofsetbacks:Howfailurecanimpr
Scienceofsetbacks:Howfailurecanimpr
Scienceofsetbacks:Howfailurecanimpr
[originaltext]Machinesthatwalk,speakandfeelarenolongersciencefiction.
A、Thepreferenceforscience.B、Theteachingmethods.C、Theteachingsubjects.D
[originaltext]Adultswholackbasicscienceandmathsskillsriskbeing"de
随机试题
OneadvantageofBLIisthatitcouldallowresearcherstoseehowatreatmenti
凡在实验室制作的马歇尔试件,高度超出误差规定都应视为废试件。()
下列根尖发育完成的恒牙牙体急性损伤中最容易并发牙髓坏死的是A.突出性脱位 B.
A.子宫性闭经 B.卵巢性闭经 C.垂体性闭经 D.下丘脑性闭经 E.以
下列各项,不属心脏听诊内容的是A:心律 B:心率 C:额外心音 D:枪击音
按照工程建设项目发展的内在规律,投资建设一个工程项目都要经过投资决策和建设实施两
断路器位置指示灯串联电阻是为了防止灯泡过电压。
每股收益最大化的缺点是没有考虑()。A:每股盈余取得的时间 B:利润和投入资本
甲油田是增值税一般纳税人,以1个月为一个纳税期缴纳增值税和资源税,2021年5月
活动隔墙与玻璃隔墙每批应至少抽查20%,并不得少于()间。A.3 B.4
最新回复
(
0
)