首页
登录
职称英语
By now, it should come as no surprise when scientists discover yet another c
By now, it should come as no surprise when scientists discover yet another c
游客
2023-12-13
54
管理
问题
By now, it should come as no surprise when scientists discover yet another case of experience changing the brain. From the sensory information we absorb to the movements we make, our lives leave footprints on the bumps and fissures of our cortex, so much so that experiences can alter "hard-wired" brain structures. Through rehab, stroke patients can coax a region of the motor cortex on the opposite side of the damaged region to pinch-hit, restoring lost mobility; volunteers who are blindfolded for just five days can reprogram their visual cortex to process sound and touch.
Still, scientists have been surprised at how deeply culture—the language we speak, the values we absorb—shapes the brain, and are rethinking findings derived from studies of Westerners. To take one recent example, a region behind the forehead called the medial prefrontal cortex supposedly represents the self: it is active when we ("we" being the Americans in the study) think of our own identity and traits. But with Chinese volunteers, the results were strikingly different. The "me" circuit hummed not only when they thought whether a particular adjective described themselves, but also when they considered whether it described their mother. The Westerners showed no such overlap between self and mom. Depending whether one lives in a culture that views the self as autonomous and unique or as connected to and part of a larger whole, this neural circuit takes on quite different functions.
"Cultural neuroscience," as this new field is called, is about discovering such differences. Some of the findings, as with the "me/mom" circuit, buttress longstanding notions of cultural differences. For instance, it is a cultural cliche that Westerners focus on individual objects while East Asians pay attention to context and background (another manifestation of the individualism-collectivism split). Sure enough, when shown complex, busy scenes, Asian-Americans and non-Asian—Americans recruited different brain regions. The Asians showed more activity in areas that process figure-ground relations—holistic context—while the Americans showed more activity in regions that recognize objects.
Psychologist Nalini Ambady of Tufts found something similar when she and colleagues showed drawings of people in a submissive pose (head down, shoulders hunched) or a dominant one (arms crossed, face forward) to Japanese and Americans. The brain’s dopamine-fueled reward circuit became most active at the sight of the stance—dominant for Americans, submissive for Japanese—that each volunteer’s culture most values, they reported in 2009. This raises an obvious chicken-and-egg question.
Cultural neuroscience wouldn’t be making waves if it found neurobiological bases only for well-known cultural differences. It is also uncovering the unexpected. For instance, a 2006 study found that native Chinese speakers use a different region of the brain to do simple arithmetic (3 + 4) or decide which number is larger than native English speakers do, even though both use Arabic numerals. The Chinese use the circuits that process visual and spatial information and plan movements (the latter may be related to the use of the abacus). But English speakers use language circuits. It is as if the West conceives numbers as just words, but the East imbues them with symbolic, spatial freight. "One would think that neural processes involving basic mathematical computations are universal," says Ambady, but they "seem to be culture-specific. "
Not to be the skunk at this party, but I think it’s important to ask whether neuroscience reveals anything more than we already know from, say, anthropology. For instance, it’s well known that East Asian cultures prize the collective over the individual, and that Americans do the opposite.
Ambady thinks cultural neuroscience does advance understanding. Take the me/mom finding, which, she argues, "attests to the strength of the overlap between self and people close to you in collectivistic cultures and the separation in individualistic cultures. It is important to push the analysis to the level of the brain. " Especially when it shows how fundamental cultural differences are—so fundamental, perhaps, that "universal" notions such as human rights, democracy, and the like may be no such thing. [br] What does "buttress" mean in the third paragraph?
选项
A、contradict
B、doubt
C、are unrelated to
D、support
答案
D
解析
原文第二段中the“me/mom”circuit的发现,表明文化背景和价值观会改变大脑结构。所以第三段中的“buttress”应该指“支持、强化”之意。
转载请注明原文地址:http://tihaiku.com/zcyy/3269004.html
相关试题推荐
ScientistssaidThursdaythatanewAIDSvaccine,thefirsteverdeclaredto
WhenscientistsattheAustralianInstituteofSportrecentlydecidedtoche
WhenscientistsattheAustralianInstituteofSportrecentlydecidedtoche
WhenscientistsattheAustralianInstituteofSportrecentlydecidedtoche
WhenscientistsattheAustralianInstituteofSportrecentlydecidedtoche
Nooneshouldbeforcedtowearauniformunderanycircumstance.Uniforms
Nooneshouldbeforcedtowearauniformunderanycircumstance.Uniforms
Nooneshouldbeforcedtowearauniformunderanycircumstance.Uniforms
Nooneshouldbeforcedtowearauniformunderanycircumstance.Uniforms
Wemusthavereceivedtheletteryesterday,butitdidn’tarrive.must——shouldmus
随机试题
Iwanttodiscusssomeideasabouthowwemanageunderground【B1】______.Ifw
Anewstudysuggeststhatthemoreteenagerswatchtelevision,themorelike
Inoursociety,wemustcommunicatewithotherpeople.Agreatdealofcommu
[originaltext]W:It’sgoodtoseeyouagain.Whatareyoudoingthesedays?You
基坑内应设置作业人员上下坡道或爬梯,数量不应少于()个。作业位置的安全通道应畅通
睡眠呼吸紊乱疾病不包括A.鼾症 B.单纯性打鼾 C.睡眠低通气综合征 D.
采用后张法施工,如设计无规定,张拉预应力筋时,要求混凝土的强度至少要达到设计规定
2018年6月,A公司拟将其拥有的某厂房转让给B公司,委托C评估机构进行了评估。
孟子曰:“民为贵,社稷次之,君为轻。是故得乎丘民而为天子,得乎天子为诸侯,得乎诸
根据反垄断法律制度的规定,反垄断执法机构在认定其他协同行为时,应考虑的因素有(
最新回复
(
0
)