首页
登录
职称英语
Where does morality come from? Throughout the history of Western civilizatio
Where does morality come from? Throughout the history of Western civilizatio
游客
2023-12-07
57
管理
问题
Where does morality come from? Throughout the history of Western civilization thinkers have usually answered either that it comes from God, or else through the application of reason.
But in The Bonobo and the Atheist, primatologist Frans de Waal argues that there’s another answer that fits the data better: morality comes from our evolutionary past as a social primate. Like our closest relatives, the apes, humans evolved in small, tightly knit, cooperative groups. As a result, again like the apes, we are exquisitely sensitive to one another’s moods, needs and intentions.
This well-developed empathy provided the trellis on which morality later flowered. De Waal, who is based at Emory University in Atlanta, Georgia, has been making this case eloquently for many years and over several books, notably in Good Natured back in 1997, and in Primates and Philosophers, 12 years later.
In his new work, he bolsters the argument by drawing on a lot of new research, carefully footnoted for those who want to dig deeper. De Waal distinguishes two degrees of morality. The first he calls "one-on-one morality", which governs how an individual can expect to be treated, and the second "community concern", a larger, more abstract concept that extends to the harmony of the group as a whole.
Chimps and bonobos certainly have the former—they respect ownership, for example, and expect to be treated according to their place in the hierarchy. But de Waal presents several examples—such as a chimp stepping in to stop a fight between two others—that suggest that they also have a rudimentary form of the latter.
The book’s title, incidentally, draws on bonobos because they are more likely than chimps to behave morally, to have concern for each other, to value harmony and so on. This, imagines, de Waal, is something morally inclined atheists would want to emulate. If humans inherited morality from our ancestors, though, what are we to make of religion? Here de Waal moves into the territory he has not explored before. Clearly, religion must do something important, since every human culture has it. But instead of religion giving us morality, de Waal turns the tables. Morality, he argues, probably gave us religion as a way of reinforcing the pre-existing community concern.
If he’s right, then there may be no absolute code of right and wrong out there to be discovered. Instead, each individual’s evolved sense of empathy and concern for the group may help shape the group’s consensus on what kind of behaviour is appropriate. In short, says de Waal, morality may be something we all have to work out together. It’s a persuasive argument, and de Waal’s cautious and evidence-based approach is one that many New Scientist readers are sure to find congenial.
That careful approach is less evident in another book covering some of the same ground. In How Animals Grieve, anthropologist Barbara King sets out to explore the question of whether non-human animals grieve for their dead. It’s an intriguing question, but unfortunately King’s book is largely a succession of anecdotes: the cat who roams the house, crying, in search of its dead litter mate; the dog who waits daily at the train station for its dead master; a dolphin trying to keep her dead calf afloat for days.
Some of these stories make a persuasive case for some animals—especially apes, elephants and cetaceans— sometimes grieving. No surprises there: I suspect most readers would have conceded that ground right from the start.
But King makes little effort to dig any deeper by exploring, for example, the neural machinery and cognitive skills an animal needs in order to be capable of grief. After all, solitary species such as cats have less need for empathy- and its corollary, grief—than social animals, and small-brained creatures such as turtles may simply lack the brainpower or not form lasting pair bonds.
To his credit, de Waal takes full note of such distinctions; King, not so much. [br] About religion and morality, Frans de Waal believes that
选项
A、religion brings morality to us.
B、no code of right and wrong can be discovered out there.
C、people who have a religious belief behave appropriately.
D、individuals who care about a group can create morality.
答案
D
解析
细节题。第八段第二句提到,个体对群体所产生的共鸣与关注,有助于形成群体对恰当行为的共识,也就是说道德是我们共同努力的结果,故[D]为答案。
转载请注明原文地址:http://tihaiku.com/zcyy/3251970.html
相关试题推荐
TheUnitedKingdomislocatedinA、northernEurope.B、westernEurope.C、northwest
ThefirstpresidentintheU.S.historywhoresignedbecauseofascandalis____
WASTstandsfor______.A、WesternAustraliaSupplyandTransportB、WebsterAptitu
TheUnitedKingdomislocatedinA、northernEurope.B、westernEurope.C、northwest
TheoriesofHistoryⅠ.Howmuchweknowabouthistory?A.W
TheoriesofHistoryⅠ.Howmuchweknowabouthistory?A.W
TheoriesofHistoryⅠ.Howmuchweknowabouthistory?A.W
TheoriesofHistoryⅠ.Howmuchweknowabouthistory?A.W
TheMarshallPlanofferedeconomicaidto______.A、EuropeancountriesB、WesternE
SomeTheoriesofHistoryI.Theproblemsofunderstandi
随机试题
Aestheticthoughtofadistinctivelymodernartemergedduringthe18thcen
企业信息化的基本内容包括基础层面,组织层面和应用层面。以下属于组织层面内容的是
企业合并取得长期股权投资,在合并中发生的各项直接相关费用应借记的会计科目是:A:
A.一线和二线抗结核药混合使用 B.2HRZE(S)/4HR C.2HRZ/
学生培养酵母菌,定期测量酵母菌的种群数量,最后画出了酵母菌种群增长的“S”型曲线
Coombs试验是诊断哪一类溶血性贫血最重要的试验A.膜缺陷B.酶缺陷C.血红蛋
(2016年11月)( )是要求评定者根据某一工作行为发生的频率或次数的多少,
汽轮机叶轮由静止开始作等加速转动。轮上M点离轴心为0.4m,在某瞬时其加速度的大
作用于膜受体的激素有A.肾上腺素 B.甲状腺素 C.生长因子 D.胰岛素
(2017年真题)近年来,国内调味品企业面临着激烈的竞争压力:其一,海外调味品企
最新回复
(
0
)