首页
登录
职称英语
Where does morality come from? Throughout the history of Western civilizatio
Where does morality come from? Throughout the history of Western civilizatio
游客
2023-12-07
54
管理
问题
Where does morality come from? Throughout the history of Western civilization thinkers have usually answered either that it comes from God, or else through the application of reason.
But in The Bonobo and the Atheist, primatologist Frans de Waal argues that there’s another answer that fits the data better: morality comes from our evolutionary past as a social primate. Like our closest relatives, the apes, humans evolved in small, tightly knit, cooperative groups. As a result, again like the apes, we are exquisitely sensitive to one another’s moods, needs and intentions.
This well-developed empathy provided the trellis on which morality later flowered. De Waal, who is based at Emory University in Atlanta, Georgia, has been making this case eloquently for many years and over several books, notably in Good Natured back in 1997, and in Primates and Philosophers, 12 years later.
In his new work, he bolsters the argument by drawing on a lot of new research, carefully footnoted for those who want to dig deeper. De Waal distinguishes two degrees of morality. The first he calls "one-on-one morality", which governs how an individual can expect to be treated, and the second "community concern", a larger, more abstract concept that extends to the harmony of the group as a whole.
Chimps and bonobos certainly have the former—they respect ownership, for example, and expect to be treated according to their place in the hierarchy. But de Waal presents several examples—such as a chimp stepping in to stop a fight between two others—that suggest that they also have a rudimentary form of the latter.
The book’s title, incidentally, draws on bonobos because they are more likely than chimps to behave morally, to have concern for each other, to value harmony and so on. This, imagines, de Waal, is something morally inclined atheists would want to emulate. If humans inherited morality from our ancestors, though, what are we to make of religion? Here de Waal moves into the territory he has not explored before. Clearly, religion must do something important, since every human culture has it. But instead of religion giving us morality, de Waal turns the tables. Morality, he argues, probably gave us religion as a way of reinforcing the pre-existing community concern.
If he’s right, then there may be no absolute code of right and wrong out there to be discovered. Instead, each individual’s evolved sense of empathy and concern for the group may help shape the group’s consensus on what kind of behaviour is appropriate. In short, says de Waal, morality may be something we all have to work out together. It’s a persuasive argument, and de Waal’s cautious and evidence-based approach is one that many New Scientist readers are sure to find congenial.
That careful approach is less evident in another book covering some of the same ground. In How Animals Grieve, anthropologist Barbara King sets out to explore the question of whether non-human animals grieve for their dead. It’s an intriguing question, but unfortunately King’s book is largely a succession of anecdotes: the cat who roams the house, crying, in search of its dead litter mate; the dog who waits daily at the train station for its dead master; a dolphin trying to keep her dead calf afloat for days.
Some of these stories make a persuasive case for some animals—especially apes, elephants and cetaceans— sometimes grieving. No surprises there: I suspect most readers would have conceded that ground right from the start.
But King makes little effort to dig any deeper by exploring, for example, the neural machinery and cognitive skills an animal needs in order to be capable of grief. After all, solitary species such as cats have less need for empathy- and its corollary, grief—than social animals, and small-brained creatures such as turtles may simply lack the brainpower or not form lasting pair bonds.
To his credit, de Waal takes full note of such distinctions; King, not so much. [br] Frans de Waal believes that morality comes from
选项
A、our primate ancestors.
B、social revolutions.
C、the ability of reasoning.
D、cooperative groups.
答案
A
解析
细节题。由题干中的人名和morality comes from定位到第二段第一句“But in The Bonobo and theAtheist,primatologist Frans de Waal argues that there’s another answer that fits the data better:moralitycomes from our evolutionary past as a social primate.”,可见Frans de Waal认为,作为灵长类动物,道德是由我们自身进化而来的,也就是说道德来自我们灵长类动物的祖先,故[A]为答案。
转载请注明原文地址:http://tihaiku.com/zcyy/3251968.html
相关试题推荐
TheoriesofHistoryⅠ.Howmuchweknowabouthistory?A.W
TheoriesofHistoryⅠ.Howmuchweknowabouthistory?A.W
TheoriesofHistoryⅠ.Howmuchweknowabouthistory?A.W
TheoriesofHistoryⅠ.Howmuchweknowabouthistory?A.W
TheoriesofHistoryⅠ.Howmuchweknowabouthistory?A.W
SomeTheoriesofHistoryI.Theproblemsofunderstandi
SomeTheoriesofHistoryI.Theproblemsofunderstandi
SomeTheoriesofHistoryI.Theproblemsofunderstandi
SomeTheoriesofHistoryI.Theproblemsofunderstandi
SomeTheoriesofHistoryI.Theproblemsofunderstandi
随机试题
[originaltext]Arecentstudyoverturnedwhatwethinkweknowaboutlying.
[originaltext]W:Sam,couldyoureviewtheresultsofthesurveyonleisurespo
有一装运液化石油气和其他易燃易爆化学物品的专用码头布置在某港区的独立安全地段。该
打印质量好、速度快,且工作原理与复印机相似的打印机是()。A.行式打印机
下面哪项是妇女保健工作的组织机构A、人事处 B、妇幼保健处 C、行政处
下列病证,不宜进行拔罐的是A.闪挫扭伤 B.瘀血痹阻 C.溃疡患处 D.伤
申请QDII资格的证券公司,净资本不低于()亿元人民币。A.5 B.6
设计和开发评审是为了()。 A.寻找类似的设计 B.评价产品的功能和
标准二十分,平均数为(),标准差为()。 (A)12、5(B)11、4
道路运输行政诉讼的定义中包含的要件有( )。A.原告是道路运输行政管理相对人
最新回复
(
0
)