首页
登录
职称英语
Where does morality come from? Throughout the history of Western civilizatio
Where does morality come from? Throughout the history of Western civilizatio
游客
2023-12-04
16
管理
问题
Where does morality come from? Throughout the history of Western civilization thinkers have usually answered either that it comes from God, or else through die application of reason.
But in The Bonobo and the Atheist, primatologist Frans de Waal argues that there’s another answer that fits the data better: morality comes from our evolutionary past as a social primate(灵长目动物). Like our closest relatives, the apes, humans evolved in small, tightly knit, cooperative groups. As a result, again like the apes, we are exquisitely sensitive to one another’s moods, needs and intentions.
This well-developed empathy provided the trellis(框架)on which morality later flowered. De Waal, who is based at Emory University in Atlanta, Georgia, has been making this case eloquently for many years and over several books, notably in Good Natured back in 1997, and in Primates and Philosophers, 12 years later.
In his new work, he bolsters(支持)the argument by drawing on a lot of new research, carefully footnoted for those who want to dig deeper. De Waal distinguishes two degrees of morality. The first he calls "one-on-one morality", which governs how an individual can expect to be treated, and the second "community concern" , a larger, more abstract concept that extends to the harmony of the group as a whole.
Chimps and bonobos certainly have the former—they respect ownership, for example, and expect to be treated according to their place in the hierarchy. But de Waal presents several examples—such as a chimp stepping in to stop a fight between two others—that suggest that they also have a rudimentary(初步的)form of the latter.
The book’s title, incidentally, draws on bonobos because they are more likely than chimps to behave morally, to have concern for each other, to value harmony and so on. This, imagines, de Waal, is something morally inclined atheists would want to emulate. If humans inherited morality from our ancestors, though, what are we to make of religion? Here de Waal moves into the territory he has not explored before. Clearly, religion must do something important, since every human culture has it. But instead of religion giving us morality, de Waal turns the tables. Morality, he argues, probably gave us religion as a way of reinforcing the pre-existing community concern.
If he’s right, then there may be no absolute code of right and wrong out there to be discovered. Instead, each individual’s evolved sense of empathy and concern for the group may help shape the group’s consensus on what kind of behaviour is appropriate. In short, says de Waal, morality may be something we all have to work out together. It’s a persuasive argument, and de Waal’s cautious and evidence-based approach is one that many New Scientist readers are sure to find congenial.
That careful approach is less evident in another book covering some of the same ground. In How Animals Grieve, anthropologist Barbara King sets out to explore the question of whether non-human animals grieve for their dead. It’s an intriguing question, but unfortunately King’s book is largely a succession of anecdotes: the cat who roams the house, crying, in search of its dead litter mate: the dog who waits daily at the train station for its dead master: a dolphin trying to keep her dead calf afloat for days.
Some of these stories make a persuasive case for some animals—especially apes, elephants and cetaceans— sometimes grieving. No surprises there: I suspect most readers would have conceded that ground right from the start.
But King makes little effort to dig any deeper by exploring, for example, the neural machinery and cognitive skills an animal needs in order to be capable of grief. After all, solitary species such as cats have less need for empathy—and its corollary, grief—than social animals, and small-brained creatures such as turtles may simply lack the brainpower or not form lasting pair bonds.
To his credit, de Waal takes full note of such distinctions: King, not so much. [br] The author writes the passage to______.
选项
A、analyze the origin of morality
B、make comments on two books
C、explore some animal behaviors
D、present some intriguing findings
答案
B
解析
主旨题。纵观全文,文章第一段提出问题:道德从何而来,第二至八段介绍Frans de Waal关于道德的研究,第九至十一段介绍Barbara King对动物是否为同类的死亡表示哀痛的研究及著作,最后一段对两个学者的著作进行比较和评价,由此可知,作者的写作意图主要是对两本书做出比较和评价,因此[B]为答案。
转载请注明原文地址:https://tihaiku.com/zcyy/3242613.html
相关试题推荐
ThefirstimmigrantsinAmericanhistorycamefrom______.A、EnglandB、theNetherl
The"IronLady"MargaretThatcherinBritishhistorywastheleaderof______Part
Itisin______thatTrumanconvincedtheWesternEuropeanpowerstojointheNort
FrancisBacon’s______isregardedasamilestoneinthehistoryofEnglishprose
InWesternCulturesyoudon’topenacloseddoorwithoutknocking,unlessit
InWesternCulturesyoudon’topenacloseddoorwithoutknocking,unlessit
InWesternCulturesyoudon’topenacloseddoorwithoutknocking,unlessit
InWesternCulturesyoudon’topenacloseddoorwithoutknocking,unlessit
InAmericanliteraryhistory,______isthefirstblackfemalepoetwhopublishe
TheGrandCanyon,carvedbytheColoradoRiver,innorthwestern______isoneofn
随机试题
Businessandgovernmentleadersconsidertheinflationratetobeanimporta
[originaltext]M:WhereisLilygoing?Isshegoingonadate?W:(1)Lilyiseigh
下列形态中,属于反转形态的是()。A、三重顶B、三角形C、矩形D、旗形A比较典型的反转形态有头肩形、双重顶(M头)、双重底(w底)、三重顶、三重
患儿,女,4个月,肺炎入院。医嘱给予心电监护,安静状态下患儿生命体征如图所示。护
下列各项中,不属于气血不足证常见脉象的是A.虚 B.细 C.涩 D.微
延胡索来源于A.毛茛科B.唇形科C.罂粟科D.藜科E.防己科
12、使用钳形电流表测量高压电缆各相电流,电缆头线间距离应大于(),且绝
企业涉及现金收支的资产负债表日后调整事项,应当调整报告年度资产负债表货币资金项目
根据个人所得税法律制度的规定,下列各项中,不属于专项附加扣除的是()。A.
(2016年真题)根据《中华人民共和国渔业法》,该法适用于中华人民共和国的(
最新回复
(
0
)